Simple Vandals or a Unique Social Movement?
A Psycho-Sociological Discourse on Internet Trolls
Milan, 2001
University of Statale of Milan Department of Sociology,
Post-Graduate Studies
status: draft
author: jf3141592
Abstract
Internet trolling has been around since the earliest days of Usenet, although the art of
Polemic discourse from which it arose can of course be traced back to the ancient Greeks.
General social consensus at the time of this writing is that in a medium free from simple
repressive censorship such as the Internet, trolling is an unwelcome yet unavoidable
aspect of modern communications. The application of collective behavioral research
techniques, however, show a possibly emerging social movement, complete with the
substantial controversy that ironically mirrors that surrounding Ptolemy's own work. This
paper shall make inquiry into the anthropological forces at work in this changing medium
and evaluate the possibility of current trends to develop into a fully realized social
movement based on unwelcome discourse and the possible effects of repression from the
conservative majority.
Contents
1. - Definitions
2. - Methodology and Research
3. - Troll Sub-Types
3.1 - The Classic
3.2 - The Obsessive
3.3 - The Abstract
3.4 - The Malicious
4. - Causative Influences
4.1 - Pre-Adolescent Conditions
4.2 - Adolescent Conditions
4.3 - Post-Adolescent, Pre-Adult Conditions
4.4 - Adult/Workplace Conditions
5. - Subject Interviews
5.1 - cyb
5.2 - uncombed
5.3 - bakunin
5.4 - Charlie
5.5 - Tommy Gun
6. - Sociological Subset Development
6.1 - Collective Behavior
6.2 - Social Movement
6.3 - Reactive Repression
7. - Conclusion
Appendix
a. Aggregate Group Data
b. Survey Results
Bibliography
1 - Definitions
A reasonably thorough understanding of the Internet, Usenet, the World Wide Web,
Instant Messengers, and other popular forms of computer-based communications is
assumed. It is beyond the scope of this paper to provide an adequate tutorial on these
subjects. Definitions for unfamiliar terms may be found in the New Hacker's Dictionary
Jargon File.
2 - Methodology and Research
Far from being neutral artifacts and physical practices from which one can
"float free", technologies are constructed in social contexts. Technologies
are developed within specific historical and cultural contexts and are
interpreted and experienced within the context of specific power
relations.[1]
Research for this paper began in the Spring of 1998 and continued for approximately 9
months. The author spent much of this time reading newsgroups and web-based
discussion forums and engaging in conversations with trolls in these mediums, along with
real-time communication via AIM and ICQ. The predominant newsgroups explored were
alt.syntax.tactical and alt.fan.karl-malden.nose. Other newsgroups and several IRC
channels were also utilized, although at the request of the interview subjects this paper
will refrain from mentioning which ones.
Due to the nature of the inquiries it was often quite difficult to obtain reliable information
from voluntary sources. Many people were very forthcoming and seemed to genuinely
enjoy the interaction, however, as the topic itself is inherently mischievous and involves
intentional deception, many responses to questions had to be discarded due to dubious
truthfulness. Every effort has been made to verify all results and data, but it is hoped that
further study can be done in a more controlled environment. Many decisions regarding
the validity of responses were entirely subjective.
A total of 58 individuals were interviewed during the course of the research. Of these, 7
were determined to be completely fictitious people, and another 4 were revealed to be
people previously interviewed under different handles (the subjects revealed this
themselves). Of the 7 fictitious subjects, 3 were also discovered to have already been
interviewed under an alias. A total of 14 subjects became hostile prior to satisfactory
completion of the interview process. As a result, the author had to change email accounts
3 times, a personal web page of hers was defaced, and she has been impersonated
countless times on Usenet, IRC, and various web-based chat rooms during the research
phase.
A total sample size of 33 individuals was used to generate the dataset. The number of
"active" trolls (defined as someone who starts or contributes to a troll more than 5 times
a week, and engages in trolling not less than 3 hours per week) at any one time is
estimated to be between 3000 and 5000 people, with the statistical sample size being
representative of approximately 0.88% of the group. The number of people who engage
in sporadic trolling is estimated at approximately 100 times the number of "active" trolls.
In addition to typical demographic profiling, subjects were asked to complete online
versions of the Meyer's-Briggs Temperament Indicator and the full MMPI-2.
3 - Troll Subtypes
troll v.,n. 1. To utter a posting on Usenet designed to attract predictable
responses or flames; or, the post itself. Derives from the phrase "trolling
for newbies" which in turn comes from mainstream "trolling", a style of
fishing in which one trails bait through a likely spot hoping for a bite. The
well-constructed troll is a post that induces lots of newbies and flamers to
make themselves look even more clueless than they already do, while
subtly conveying to the more savvy and experienced that it is in fact a
deliberate troll. If you don't fall for the joke, you get to be in on it. See
also YHBT. 2. An individual who chronically trolls in sense 1; regularly
posts specious arguments, flames or personal attacks to a newsgroup,
discussion list, or in email for no other purpose than to annoy someone or
disrupt a discussion. Trolls are recognizable by the fact that the have no
real interest in learning about the topic at hand - they simply want to utter
flame bait. Like the ugly creatures they are named after, they exhibit no
redeeming characteristics, and as such, they are recognized as a lower
form of life on the net, as in, "Oh, ignore him, he's just a troll."[2]
The Jargon File definition is widely accepted as the official one for Internet trolls. It also
contains information on other related topics like Troll-O-Meter (used to measure the
quality of a troll), and troglodyte (a malicious troll). For the purposes of this paper it will
be necessary to further refine definitions into 4 unique sub-sets of trolls, although there is
much overlap and very few trolls fall squarely into one particular category only.
3.1 - The Classic
The "Classic" type troll is the most polemic, in that the primary purpose is to engage
others in a heated discussion about a topic. Classicists tend to have above average
intelligence, and the predominant Meyers-Briggs temperament was eNFp.
Their disorders tend towards the cyclothymic type, with many becoming excessively
involved in pleasurable activities with lack of concern for the high potential of painful
consequences alternating with restriction of involvement in pleasurable activities and
guilt over past activities.[3]
Interestingly, all subjects who fit the Classic profile were either British or Canadian with
the exception of one woman from S. Florida. Analyses revealed the British participants
generated more contextual causes for events, but also had a stronger self-serving bias
than the Canada participants. Further, each cultural group viewed achievement events as
more controllable than interpersonal events, but the Canada sample differentiated
between achievement and interpersonal events more strongly than did the British sample.
This closely matches the findings demonstrated by N.C. Higgins and G. Bhatt in Culture
moderates the self-serving bias: Etic and emic features of causal attributions, in that
causal explanations for life events in the two cultures do possess both etic (i.e., universal)
and emic (i.e., culture-specific) features.[4]
3.2 - The Obsessive
The "Obsessive" type troll's personality disorders obviously tend towards obsessive-
compulsive, yet the predominant PType was actually found to be that of Artistic.
Kiersey's note that in them "may be found an instinctive longing for the natural, the
pastoral, the bucolic. They are quite at home in the wilds, and nature seems to welcome
them" held true in most cases.[5] Sixty-four percent of the study group closely fit the
eStP type.
The Obsessives tend to fixate on one particular topic, basing all their trolls from a
common theme. The mental models underlying adult attachment styles were
conceptualized from a social cognitive perspective. Results showed that whereas most
people reported experience with multiple styles of relating, the general attachment style
they endorsed was related to: a) the proportion of their significant relationships in which
their feelings corresponded to the different attachment style descriptions, b) the ease with
which they could generate exemplar relationships to match these descriptions, and c)
their interpersonal expectations in these relationships. Overall, the findings suggest that
most people process relational knowledge corresponding to all three attachment styles
and that the relative availability and accessibility of this knowledge determines which
style people report to characterize their thinking about relationships.
It is important to differentiate between the Obsessive and the spamming activities of the
Malicious, as the compulsive influences in the former are contingent upon an inherent
creative need whereby the troll is compelled to adulate the object of his obsession with
ever more elaborate tributes.[6]
3.3 - The Abstract
Perhaps one of the most interesting types of trolls is what shall be referred to here as
"Abstract". They are known to produce long, stream of conscious rants that frequently
appear to devolve into utter nonsense. Close inspection of 42 unique Abstract trolls,
however, revealed a stunning degree of complexity and duplicitous meaning hidden
within the words, calling to mind Dadaist works by Joyce and Lucas. One particularly
lengthy rant was actually a fairly insightful deconstruction of Hasidic numerology as
compared to Pagan cabalistic works.
Meyers-Briggs results were 23% iStP, with another 15% being yHBt, and all showing a
sense of entitlement, i.e., unreasonable expectations of especially favorable treatment or
automatic compliance with his or her expectations, which is strongly indicative of a
narcissistic personality.[7] This narcissism was further shown to tend towards an
authority complex.
Interviews demonstrate that an actor who performs likable behaviors towards superiors
(persons on whom the actor depends), and dislikable behaviors towards subordinates
(persons the actor does not depend on), is judged as extremely dislikable and slimy.
Subsequent experiments address several theoretical accounts of this Slime effect . Likable
behaviors towards superiors induce suspicion of ulterior motivation; this suspicion is
gradually confirmed as more dislikable behaviors towards subordinates are encountered
(Exp. 3). The operation of a Slime schema is indicated by the emergence of an illusory
correlation between an actor's behavior and the power of the behavioral target, such that
the actor is erroneously perceived as more likable towards superiors (Exp. 4). Further,
perceivers spontaneously discern the behavioral pattern of licking upward, kicking
downwardly, regardless of processing time (Exp 5). The discussion addresses the
implications of these results with respect to impression formation and inconsistency
resolution, trait inferences and correspondence bias, and lay theories of self-
presentational behavior.[8]
3.4 - The Malicious
"Malicious" trolls tended towards strong destructive actions such as spamming and
excessive profanity. They showed no tendencies towards any of the Meyers-Briggs
known types, instead only indicating significant personality defect PTypes such as
histrionics and compensatory narcissism. Other issues were raised with such surprising
regularity that one must conclude that virtually all Malicious trolls have both erectile
dysfunction and suffer frequent enuresis The average IQ of this group was 83.
Another thing unique to this group was a concentration of ages not apparent in the others.
The youngest of these was 12, the oldest 16, and the average being 15.27 years of age. As
is to be expected of this age group, they continued to show a markedly absent sense of
maturity and a significantly diminished grasp of rudimentary social skills.
The author also had the opportunity to interview a former Malicious, who chose to go by
the moniker "Vlad" in our ICQ discussions. Vlad had stopped trolling some 10 years ago,
yet according to the MMPI-2, all of the previously mentioned personality disorders were
still present, and IQ had actually decreased.
4 - Causative Influences
The causative influences leading to trolling were similar for all groups and are as follows:
4.1 - Preadolescent Conditions
Pre-adolescent conditions were strongly marked by cases of Munchausen syndrome[9],
characterized by the feigning of the symptoms of a disease or injury in order to undergo
diagnostic tests, hospitalization, or medical or surgical treatment. They frequently found
reason to stay home from school and watch Captain Kangaroo.
The first three troll types also were preoccupied with members of the opposite sex at a
young age, often finding themselves being punished for playing "doctor". The malicious
type, on the other hand, maintained a firm belief in girls having "cooties" until well into
their late teens.[10]
4.2 Adolescent Conditions
Adolescent conditions were identical for all groups, proving to hold quite true to Shaw &
McKay's study of Juvenile Delinquency[11]
In their Juvenile Delinquency and Urban Areas (1942), Shaw & McKay applied the
Concentric Zone Model developed by Park & Burgess to the study of juvenile
delinquency. They examined arrest rates of juveniles throughout the city of Chicago
during the years 1900-06, 1917-23, and 1927-33. These were years of high immigration,
meaning that immigrant groups rapidly "migrated" from the inner city towards the
suburbs, as the older and more established immigrant groups were pushed along by the
arrival of poorer immigrant groups, who took their place in the center of the city. By
comparing the rates from three different time periods, Shaw & McKay believed they
could show whether delinquency was caused by particular immigrant groups or by the
environment in which immigrants lived. That is:
? if high delinquency rates for particular immigrant groups remained high during their migration
through the city's different ecological environments, then delinquency could be associated with
their distinctive constitutional or cultural features;
? if delinquency rates decreased as immigrants moved through different ecological environments,
then delinquency could not be associated with the particular constitution of the immigrants, but
must somehow be connected with their environment.
Dividing three maps of Chicago into even grids, Shaw & McKay shaded each square
darker or lighter to indicate its average arrest rate over the three time periods: 1900-06,
1917-23, and 1927-33.
Comparing the maps, Shaw & McKay recognized that the pattern of delinquency rates
not only remained constant over time, but also corresponded to the "natural urban areas"
of Park & Burgess' Concentric Zone Model. They came to the important conclusion that
delinquency rates always remained high for a certain region of the city (ecological zone
2), no matter what immigrant group lived there. Therefore, delinquency was not
"constitutional"-as Lombroso and his followers had argued -but must somehow be
correlated with the particular ecological environment in which it occurs! Shaw &
McKay's eventual explanation of this correlation is their "social disorganization theory."
4.3 - Post-Adolescent Conditions
Post-adolescent conditions for all remaining groups (none of the Malicious had reached
this stage in their development) were characterized mostly by heavy ingestion of hard
drugs.
? 97% had used Marijuana
? 74% ingested LSD
? 22% ate psychedelic mushrooms
? 5% injected heroin
? 18% snorted cocaine
? 23% used amphetamines
? 100% used ketamine on a daily basis
? Only 2 people, known as Rob M. and Jeff B. used poppers
? Opium was frequently discussed
? MDMA had, and still has, a strong following among many in the test group
? Strangely, none of the subjects ever smoked crack
4.4 - Adult/Workplace Conditions
Upon entering the workforce almost all subjects reported an intense boredom with
mundane activities, and trolling was known to increase for many. Implications for self-
peer agreement in personality judgment resulted in a high level of cognitive resonance
among most.[12]
We examined the relationship between trait ambiguity and self-peer agreement in
personality judgment. In Group 1, self-peer agreement was lower on ambiguous traits
(those with many behavioral referents) than on unambiguous ones (those with few
behavioral referents). This finding was partially moderated by the level of friendship
between peers. These results suggest that people disagree in their judgments because they
use idiosyncratic trait definitions when making judgments on ambiguous traits. Group 2
tested this explanation by exploring self-peer agreement when participant pairs were
forced to use the same trait definition versus different ones when judging themselves and
each other. Forcing participants to use the same trait definition increased the degree to
which their judgments covaried with one another. Discussion centers on the cognitive and
motivational forces that can influence the degree to which personality judgments differ.
5 - Subject Interviews
Several excepts of the interviews with various subjects have been reprinted here, as they
further understanding into the methodologies and techniques utilized by several of the
groups.
6 - Sociological Subset Development
Consider, as an example, the two dominant theoretical perspectives regarding social
movements and collective behavior: Resource mobilization, the dominant paradigm
guiding social movement research throughout the 1970s and into the 1980s, and the more
recently developed "new social movement" theory. Resource mobilization abandoned
earlier psychological approaches to collective action and introduced resources,
organizing, and rationality as the key variables explaining the emergence of social
movements and collective action.[13]
Despite the attractiveness of resource mobilization and its proven utility to social
movement scholars, important aspects of social movements remain unexplained by
resource mobilization theory.[14] The result has been the rise of new social movement
theory, which emphasizes social psychological processes, collective identity, and
continuity as complements to the manipulation of resources described in resource
mobilization theory.[15]
6.1 - Collective Behavior
The collective behavior aspects of these trolls show strong Marxist tendencies towards
the development of a sociological subset. Engels' writings on the working class in
England are particularly relevant:
A town, such as London, where a man may wander for hours together
without reaching the beginning of the end, without meeting the slightest
hint which could lead to the interference that there is open country within
reach, is a strange thing. This colossal centralization, this heaping together
of two and a half millions of human beings at one point, has multiplied the
power of this two and a half millions a hundred-fold; has raised London to
the commercial capital of the world, created the giant docks and
assembled the thousand vessels that continually cover the Thames. I know
nothing more imposing than the view which the Thames offers during the
ascent from the sea to London Bridge. The masses of buildings, the
wharves on both sides, especially from Woolwich upwards, the countless
ships along both shores, crowding ever closer together, until, at last, only a
narrow passage remains in the middle of the river, a passage through
which hundreds of steamers shoot by one another; all this so vast, so
impressive, that a man cannot collect himself, but is lost in the marvel of
England's greatness before he sets foot upon English soil.[16]
Indeed, after months of analysis it becomes plainly apparent that this collectivism in the
troll community has become that of a politically socialist group. Sharing of common
themes is encouraged amongst them, and quite a few are adherents of the Open Source
philosophy which dictates that the products of a man's labor do not belong to that person,
but are in fact a gift to the collective whole. Commerce is scoffed at, profit is disdained,
ownership of property is considered useless.
A new socialism is indeed developing in the online communities, mainly in the fringe
elements. Thus, the question becomes whether or not these tendencies have the force of
social movement or are merely to be relegated to an idle pastime that amounts to little.
6.2 - Social Movement
In contrast to the elite theorists, pluralists argued that power was actually becoming less
centralized. Pluralists saw advances in technology as providing the potential for greater
democratization and increased participation in political processes. Some scholars have
begun to observe growing attempts to utilize new communication technology, especially
computer technology, in the interests of the less privileged. In particular, as social
movement activists have become more sophisticated computer users, some of the
resources once monopolized by the "establishment" are being used to improve
communication among activists. Activists throughout the world now use their access to
established networks, via the Internet and Usenet, as well as specialized networks such as
Peacenet and Econet, to communicate about social movement activities and to form
collective action agendas.[17]
The number of trolls is undoubtedly growing along with the internet, yet the dominance
of these groups over those that they troll is what will undoubtedly create the social
movement. Their inherent superiority in intelligence, their greater wit, firmer grasp of
technological issues, and of course sizable gun collections, will lead their socialist group
from fringe element to dominant controller of society.[18]
Needless to say, there will be a great deal of resistance from many of those who stand to
lose the most from such a change in regime.
6.3 - Reactive Repression
An example of reactive repression occurred in late 1992 when Mattel
released a new Barbie doll called "Teen Talk Barbie." These dolls were
programmed to say different things that were supposed to be related to
being a teenage young woman. One sentence the teen Barbie spouted was
"Math class is tough." Recognizing that this message reinforced the
prevailing socialization of young women to fear math and to feel unable to
perform mathematical tasks, an association of women scholars mounted a
campaign to get Barbie to stop saying "Math class is tough."
Part of this attempt was an electronic mail message sent to women
academics explaining the situation, urging action, and providing names
and addresses at the Mattel headquarters. By the time this message
reached me, it had been forwarded three times and I was a member of a
list of 90 people who received the message. If the message had reached me
through a similar pattern of forwarding, it could have reached a maximum
of more than 65 million computer sites from just three forwards!
Although it is unlikely that each person who received the message
forwarded it to 90 people, the point is still clear: The ease of forwarding
messages and sending the same message to multiple sites can result in a
tremendous diffusion of information in an extremely short period of time.
The advantages of speed and ease are, however, inextricably connected
with the disadvantages of information overload. Although information
reaches thousands of nodes, the question remains whether the information
is digested by the audience or passed over like so much junk mail.[19]
What the above quote represents, more than anything else, is that people are complete
morons who will descend to levels of incredible knee-jerk stupidity at the slightest
suggestion. They will bite at anything if given 1/2 a chance. Why is this the case? Well,
I've got my own theories on that one.
Here's the deal: People are bored. Yes, bored. Bored bored bored bored bored. Most of us
have plenty to eat, roof over our heads, etc. There's no challenge in life, and as a result we
search for something important because we need to have our causes.
Real causes, of course, are damn hard to find in this day and age, and when they do
present themselves (like starving children) most people ignore them because, hey, that
shit's depressing. We want a cause that doesn't hurt to bad, but we're still going to blow it
way out of proportion and try to base our lives around it, even if it is really meaningless.
This is why people react the way they do to things that absolutely do not mean shit in a
larger sense of how the world functions.
The study examined the development of stereotypes about troll groups and their
detractors. Results showed that, at low levels of experience, stereotypic group knowledge
is derived from information about particular group exemplars. However, as experience
increases, an abstract group stereotype is formed that is stored and retrieved
independently of the exemplars upon which it was based. Results of the study suggested
that pre-existing stereotypes about well-known groups are represented as abstract
structures in memory. These results indicate that stereotypical knowledge is most likely
to be exemplar-based in the absence of abstract stereotypes. The implications of these
findings for other aspects of stereotyping and social perception are discussed.
7 - Conclusion
The diffusion models of cycles of protest as a theory of social movements do apply to
Internet trolling, and the reactive repression only further strengthens the causes.
Marxist/Loschian Central Place Theory predicts a cumulative effect in further densifying
the nodal points and their relative cascading influence on downstream medium. As a
predicator of all current discussion forums, Usenet social trends can be used to accurately
model long-term anthropological movements towards a unique, largely anarchic
paradigm.
Laskey, Klein & Wyer's recent work explores how these changes may come about in a
disparate, networked environment:
Two experiments examined the role of memory for behavioral episodes in
judgments about in-groups and out-groups. Using a minimal group
paradigm, participants read either positive or negative trait-relevant
behaviors performed by group members. They then were asked to make
judgments about the group's trait characteristics. Results demonstrated
that, for groups described positively, judgments about the out-group but
not the in-group were accomplished by retrieving from memory specific
behaviors performed by group members. By contrast, for groups described
negatively, judgments about the in-group but not the out-group were
accomplished by retrieving specific behaviors performed by group
members. These results suggest that basic differences in the way
judgments about in-groups and out-groups are made contribute to the
establishment and perpetuation of intergroup bias by decreasing the
stability of negative in-group and positive out-group impressions and
increasing the stability of positive in-group and negative out-group
impressions.[20]
and later, in another publication:
Social identity theory and related viewpoints hold that an individual's
social group memberships become part of the psychological self, with
diverse and powerful effects on thoughts, feelings, and behavior.
However, the successful tests of this hypothesis to date have mainly
involved evaluative judgments or reward allocations as dependent
measures. Adapting a method developed by Aron et al. (1991), based on
the same logic as Stroop interference, we provide more direct evidence
that an in-group can be represented as part of the self. Subjects made
speeded self-descriptiveness judgments for a variety of traits. Analyses
showed that responses were slower and involved more errors for traits on
which the individual believed he or she differed from an in-group,
compared to traits on which the individual and the in-group were
perceived as similar. Matches or mismatches between the self and a salient
out-group had no effect. This evidence suggests that cognitive
representations of the self and an in-group are directly linked, to the point
where reports about the self are facilitated for traits on which the self and
in-group are similar, and inhibited for traits involving dissimilarity.[21]
Fundamentally, this is classic class conflict. Current conventional wisdom has it that the
solution to stopping trolling from degrading civil discourse is to ignore it, yet our studies
strongly suggest that not only will this be ineffectual, it may exacerbate the problem.
Subjects displayed a degree of hubris usually unseen outside of clinical environments, yet
the work of Skinner provides for the alleviation of such excessive egoism. Applying his
work to the present day issues involves not ignoring, but challenging the assertions of
trolls, forcing them into a situation whereby the only possible outcome is their concession
to being of inferior intellect to those targeted by the initial attack. This constant negative
reinforcement can only lead to a significant reduction in the periodicity of attempted
trolling.
Appendix
a. Aggregate Group Data
b. Survey Results
Bibliography
[1] Constance Penley and Andrew Ross, eds, Technoculture. (Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press, 1991); Judy Wajcman. Feminism Confronts Technology. (University
Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University, 1991).
[2] http://www.eps.mcgill.ca/jargon/jargon.html#troll
[3] Akiskal, Hagop S. Delineating irritable and hyperthymic variants of the cyclothymic
temperament. In Journal of Personality Disorders, 6(4), pp. 326-342. New York: Guilford
Press, 1992.
[4] Higgins, N.C., & Bhatt, G. (in press). Culture moderates the self-serving bias: Etic
and emic features of causal attributions in India and in Canada. Social Behavior and
Personality, 29(1).
[5] Keirsey, David, and Marilyn Bates. Please Understand Me: Character and
Temperament Types. 3rd ed. Del Mar: Prometheus Nemesis, 1978.
[6] http://www.thegrid.net/pix/PORTMAN73A.JPG
[7] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington: Author, 1994.
[8] Vonk, R. (in press). The Slime Effect: Suspicion and dislike of likeable behavior
towards superiors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
[9] http://my.webmd.com/content/c4_asset/merriam-webster_medical_dictionary_165330
[10] http://cgi.pbs.org/wgbh/arthur/prunella/cootie/index.html
[11] http://www.crimetheory.com/Soc1/Chic2.htm
[12] Hayes, A. F., & Dunning, D. (in press). Trait ambiguity and construal processes:
Implications for self-peer agreement in personality judgment. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology.
[13] http://www.nd.edu/%7Edmyers/cbsm/vol1/myers2.html
[14] http://www.nd.edu/%7Edmyers/cbsm/vol1/myers2.html
[15] http://www.nd.edu/%7Edmyers/cbsm/vol1/myers2.html
[16] Engels, Freidrich. The Condition of the Working Class in England
[17] http://www.nd.edu/%7Edmyers/cbsm/vol1/myers2.html
[18] http://www.nd.edu/%7Edmyers/cbsm/vol2/bgham99.pdf
[19] http://www.nd.edu/%7Edmyers/cbsm/vol1/myers2.html
[20] Sherman, J. W., Klein, S. B., Laskey, A., & Wyer, N. A. (in press). Intergroup bias
in group judgment processes: The role of behavioral memories. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology.
[21] Smith, E. R., & Henry, S. (1996). An in-group becomes part of the self: Response
time evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 635-642.
               (
geocities.com/it/roccopapaleo/faq)                   (
geocities.com/it/roccopapaleo)                   (
geocities.com/it)