For Sept 17... | ||
If we know what 'normal' behavior is like, we know what to expect (prediction) of children at specific ages and can plan appropriately (control) for children's changing needs. Empirical knowledge about children's development can counter myths ('daycare is bad for children'), and theories based on empirical research (description and understanding) can inform the design of school and child care programs as well as government and private services for children. Scientifically-based knowledge of child development enables us to be more successful in advocating for children's needs. As students of child development, theories guide our
observations, framing our understanding of why children behave the way
they do at the different points in their development. While no one theory
can fully explain development, most researchers today take an eclectic
approach, incorporating aspects of various theories into their work. |
||
We reviewed the three domains of development that are
addressed by theorists, and the different historical theoretical schools
of thought concerning child development. Then we went on to modern
theoretical perspectives, including the information processing theory
of cognitive development, Vygotsky's socio-cultural theory, and ecological
theory.
We reviewed the methods used in researching child development. I mentioned the psychophysiological approach, and in addition to the methods listed last week, we added considered the aspects of sampling developmental changes over time. Obviously, one approach is to follow specific individuals over the course of their development. This is called the longitudinal approach, and while it shows the steps or stages or increments in change over time, it has several disadvantages. First, participants may become 'test-wise' over time. Also, over time, many participants may move away or drop out of the study. Longitudinal studies are more time-consuming and expensive to conduct. Finally, researcher may be contaminated by 'cohort effects', differences between groups of children born at different points in history. Development in a specific set of individuals of the same age may be influenced by environmental events or conditions that impact that particular cohort differently than from an earlier or later group of children of the same ages. For example, if you were to study the 'normal' development of nightmares, children who were four when the events of 9/11 occurred may have very different dream behaviors than children whose dream development from age 4 on were first studied ten years ago. Cross-sectional studies Look at the full range of ages being studied all at the same time. In other words, if you want to know about development between ages 8 to 12, you examine 8, 9, 10, 11 , and 12 years olds all at once and draw conclusions based on differences between these age groups. This approach is less expensive, less time consuming and easier to carry out, but does not reveal much about individual differences in development, and is also likely to be contaminated by cohort effects. (Again, 9/11 might have a distorting influence on a study of the normal development of nightmares...) The longitudinal-sequential approach looks at 2 or more age groups over a shorter period of time than a purely longitudinal design. By simultaneously studying two groups, one age 8 and one age10 over 2 years time, you can get a four year picture of development that controls, to some degree, for cohort effects. Microgenetic research gives a close-up look at individual developmental changes but is expensive and difficult to do . Ethical considerations in doing research on children. |
||
A history of childhood... 1. Our understanding of childhood as a special period,
unique in the development of a person, is relatively recent. In
western societies, up to a few hundred years ago, infants were regarded as
a unique stage of complete dependency, but once they were no longer
toddlers, they were regarded as minimally competent miniature adults: they
were expected to conform to adult expectations and to contribute to the
welfare of the household to the extent that they were able. If they lived
on a farm they gradually took on more and more responsibilities; in all
but wealthy families, children of non-farming families often had to go to
work as and apprentice to a trade or to a job doing manual and menial
labor. Even today in parts of the world, many children are at work tending
cattle or younger siblings, weaving rugs, chopping weeds in
the fields, gathering wood, etc. In wealthy families, boys, and, more
rarely, girls might go to school or have a tutor but much of their
training was how to be a 'lady' or a 'gentleman'. 2. In the sixteenth century, the Puritans developed the idea that children were 'born sinners' and that their evil nature and wild spirit had to be broken, and that the parent's job of instilling the ability to tell right from wrong in their children was their most important job. 3. A century later, with the period of Enlightenment, and of human dignity and respect, children began to be seen in a new light, not as evil, but as raw material to be shaped by the parents. John Locke, a leader in this philosophy, saw children as a 'tabula rasa' or blank slate upon which the environment would write out what the future adult would be. This line of thinking held that children should be treated with love and affection and taught reason in order to develop into rational and responsible adults. 4 .In the 18th century, the idea of children being born as 'noble savages' whose development to adulthood would unfold naturally in positive directions as long as the environment (especially parents) didn't ruin them. Jean Jacques Rousseau was the philosopher who is most closely associated with this philosophy. 5. The next stage in understanding 'childhood' came with the dawn of biological science, especially Darwin's theory of evolution. Through careful observation, Darwin saw many similarities between the early stages of growth and development in other species and while some aspects of his speculations were proven wrong, his studies lead to careful systematic study of child development. 6. Early in the 1900's, the study of child development took off as a scientific endeavor. While parents often undertook 'baby biographies', descriptions of their own children's developmental milestones, these were often biased and unfocussed in terms of specific theories. (However, Jean Piaget, a 'giant' in the early years of the science of child development, was trained as a biologist and began his work on a theory of children's cognitive development with extraordinarily detailed and detached descriptions of his own children's infancies.) 7. At the turn of the century, another approach, normative studies, were begun by G. Stanley Hall and his student, Arnold Gesell. They attempted to gather objective data about average ('normal') children's behaviors at different ages. Gesell' information served as a guide to parents for the milestones to expect at each age. |
||
I handed out the take-home test and went over the instructions. | ||
Prepare take home test (Test #1) on Chapter 1. I kept it short so that you could also read Chapter 2. |