PSYCHO
SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT OF INFANTS AND TODDLERS.
A.
ERIKSON’S THEORY OF INFANT AND TODDLER PERSONALITY
The leading neo-Freudian perspective is Erik Erikson’s psychosocial
theory.
-
Freud
called the first year the oral stage, in which infants’ need
for food and oral stimulation is vital
. Erikson believed that a healthy outcome in infancy depended on the quality
of the mother’s behavior during feeding, and not the amount of
food or oral stimulation offered. He called this period of development
Basic Trust versus Mistrust
.
Basic trust versus mistrust is the conflict during infancy in
Erikson’s psychosocial theory. The dilemma is resolved positively if
the balance of care is sympathetic and loving.
-
Freud’s second stage is the anal stage, during which
instinctual energies shift to the anal region. In Erikson’ s theory,
Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt is the conflict of toddlerhood,
when the toddler is figuring out the differences between himself and
the other important people in his life. It is resolved positively if
parents provide suitable guidance and reasonable choices.
-
If
children emerge from the first few years without sufficient trust in
caregivers and without a healthy sense of individuality, the seeds are
sown for adjustment problems.
B.
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT: Emotions play
a powerful role in organizing the developments that Erikson regarded as so
important to relationships with caregivers, exploration of the
environment, and discovery of self.
-
Basic
emotions are those that can be directly inferred from facial
expressions, such as happiness, interest, surprise, fear, anger,
sadness, and disgust. At first, a baby’s emotional life is
relatively undifferentiated, consisting of two global arousal states:
attraction to pleasant stimulation and withdrawal from unpleasant
stimulation.Over time, emotions
become clear, well-organized signals.
-
Happiness
binds parent and baby and fosters the infant’s developing
competence. The social smile—the smile evoked by the stimulus of the
human face—first appears between 6 and 10 weeks. Laughter first
appears around 3 to 4 months in response to active stimuli.
-
Anger
and Sadness: From 4 to 6 months into the second year, angry
expressions increase in frequency and intensity. Cognitive and motor
development both contribute to the increase in angry reactions with
age. Expressions of sadness are usually less frequent than anger.
Sadness is especially common when parent—infant interaction is
seriously disrupted.
-
Fear:
Like anger, fear rises during the second half of the first year.
The most frequent expression of fear is to unfamiliar adults, a
response called stranger anxiety. Stranger
anxiety depends on several factors: temperament, past experiences with
strangers, and the situation in which baby and stranger meet.
Culture can modify stranger anxiety through infant-rearing practices.
The rise in fear after 6 months of age helps protect newly crawling
and walking babies by keeping them close to caregivers and careful
about approaching unfamiliar people and objects.
-
Understanding
and Responding to the Emotions of Others: Early on, babies detect
others’ emotions through a fairly automatic process of emotional
contagion. Between 7 and 10 months, infants perceive facial
expressions as organized patterns, and they can match the emotional
tone of a voice with the appropriate face of a speaking person.
Social referencing occurs when an infant relies on a trusted
person’s emotional reaction to decide how to respond in an uncertain
situation. Social referencing provides infants with a method of
learning about the environment through indirect experience. By
toddlerhood, children use emotional signals to infer others’
internal states and guide their own actions.
-
Self-conscious
emotions appear in the middle of the second year. They involve injury
to or enhancement of the sense of self and include shame,
embarrassment, guilt, envy, and pride. Self-conscious emotions play an
important role in children’s achievement-related and moral
behaviors.
-
Beginnings
of Emotional Self-Regulation: Emotional self-regulation refers to the
strategies used to adjust emotional states to a comfortable level of
intensity. In the early months,
infants have only limited capacity to regulate their emotional states.
By the end of the first year, babies’ ability to move around permits
them to regulate feelings more effectively by approaching or
retreating from various stimuli. As caregivers help infants regulate
their emotional states, they contribute to the child’s style of
emotional self-regulation. Mothers more often match their baby’s
positive, especially with their sons, than negative emotions. Growth
in representation and language permits toddlers to talk their emotions,
an important coping mechanism in managing emotional states..
C.
DEVELOPMENT
OF TEMPERAMENT (PP. 262-269) Temperament
refers to stable individual differences in quality and intensity of
emotional reaction, activity level, attention, and emotional
self-regulation.
-
Research indicates that
temperament is predictive of psychological adjustment but that
parenting practices can modify children’s emotional styles.
-
In
one research model, there were three
basic types of temperament seen in children:
1) easy children (40 percent of sample) who quickly establish regular
routines, are cheerful, and adapt easily to new experiences, 2) difficult
children (10 percent of sample) who are irregular in daily routines,
slow to accept new experiences, and tend to react negatively and
intensely, and 3) Slow-to-warm-up children (15 percent of sample) are
inactive, have mild, low-key reactions to stimuli, and adjust slowly
to new experiences. 35 percent
of children did not fit any of these categories, demonstrating blends
of characteristics instead. The difficult temperamental type places
children at risk for adjustment problems.
-
Another
model found six types: that varied along certain dimensions. 1)
emotion (fearful distress, irritable distress, positive affect,
and soothability), 2)
attention (attention span/persistence), and 3)
action (activity level).
These components were seen as forming an integrated system of
capacities and limitations.
-
Assessments
of Physiological Reactions finds that 1) Inhibited, or shy, children
react negatively to and withdraw from novel stimuli, 2) uninhibited,
or sociable, children display positive emotion to and approach novel
stimuli, and 3) the heart rate, hormone levels, and EEG waves in the
frontal cortex differentiate children with inhibited and uninhibited
temperamental styles.
-
Many
studies support the long-term stability of temperament, yet, when the
evidence as a whole is examined carefully, temperamental stability
from one age period to the next is generally low to moderate. One
major reason that temperament is not more stable may be that
temperament itself develops with age; early behaviors reorganize into
new, more complex systems. Long-term prediction from early temperament
is best achieved from the second year of life and after.
-
Changes
shown by many children suggest that experience can modify
biologically-based temperamental traits.Genetic findings from twin
studies reveal that identical twins are more similar than fraternal
twins across a wide range of temperamental traits and personality
measures. About half of the individual differences among us can be
traced to differences in our genetic make-up.
Asian infants tend to be less active, irritable, and vocal than
Caucasian infants. Boys tend to
be more active and daring than girls.
-
Environmental
Influences: Some differences in early temperament are encouraged by
cultural beliefs and practices. For example, Asian mothers do more
comforting and Caucasian mothers more stimulating.
Parents more often encourage infant sons to be physically active and
daughters to seek help and physical closeness. Research indicates that
when one child in a family is viewed as easy, another is likely to be
perceived as difficult, even though the second child might not be very
difficult when compared to children in general.
-
Temperament
and Child Rearing: The Goodness-of-Fit Model: The
goodness-of-fit model explains how temperament and environmental
pressures work together to produce favorable outcomes.
Goodness-of-fit is an effective match between child-rearing
environments and a child’s temperament, leading to healthy
adjustment. Difficult infants
are less likely than easy babies to receive sensitive care.
( Caregiving, of course, is not just responsive to the child’s
temperament; it also depends on life conditions and cultural values.)
D.
DEVELOPMENT OF ATTACHMENT : Attachment is the strong affectional
tie that we feel toward special people in their lives. Infant attachment
behaviors include seeking closeness to the mother, following her about,
and crying and calling in her absence.
-
Psychoanalytic
theory regards feeding as the central context in which caregivers and
babies build emotional bonds. According to the behaviorist drive
reduction explanation, as the mother satisfies the baby’s hunger
(primary drive), her presence becomes a secondary or learned drive
because it is paired with tension relief.
Research indicates the attachment bond is not solely dependent on the
satisfaction of the infant’s hunger. Human infants can also become
attached to people who do not feed them, as well as to soft, cuddly
objects.
-
Today, ethological theory of attachment is the most widely accepted
view of attachment. Bowlby’s ethological theory views the infant’s
emotional tie to the mother as an evolved response that promotes
survival. The human infant is endowed with a set of built-in behaviors
that keep the parent nearby, which increases the chances that the
infant will be protected from danger.
-
The
development of attachment takes place in four phases:
-
The
preattachment phase (birth to 6 weeks)—built-in signals such
as smiling and crying help bring the newborn into close contact
with other humans.
-
The
“attachment-in-the-making” phase (6 weeks to 6-8
months)—infants begin to respond differently to a familiar
caregiver than to a stranger
-
The
phase of “clear-cut” attachment (6-8 months to 18 months-2
years)—attachment to the familiar caregiver is evident. At this
stage, babies exhibit separation anxiety when they become upset at
the departure of a familiar caregiver and they use caregivers as a
secure base from which they can explore the environment and to
which they can return for emotional security and support.
-
Formation
of a reciprocal relationship (18 months-2 years and
on)—separation anxiety decreases; instead, toddlers try to
persuade caregivers not to leave.
-
An
internal working model is a set of expectations derived from early
caregiving experiences concerning the availability and reliability of
attachment figures and their likelihood of providing support during
times of stress. This becomes a guide for all future close
relationships.
-
The
Strange Situation is a procedure for measuring the quality of
attachment between 1 and 2 years of age. It involves short separations
from and reunions with the parent.
Secure attachment characterizes infants who may or may not cry
at parental separation but are easily comforted by the parent when she
returns. Avoidant attachment
describes infants who are usually not distressed by parental
separation and who avoid the parent when she returns. Resistant
attachment identifies infants who remain close to the parent
before departure and display angry, resistive behavior when she
returns. Disorganized/disoriented
attachment characterizes infants who respond in a confused,
contradictory way when reunited with parents. This pattern seems to
reflect the greatest insecurity.
Some
points about attachment:
1. Quality of
attachment is usually secure and stable for middle-SES babies experiencing
favorable life conditions.
2. Infants who move
from insecurity to security typically have well-adjusted mothers with
positive family and friendship ties.
3. For low-SES
families with many stresses and little support, attachment status usually
moves away from security or changes from one insecure pattern to another.
4. Many children
show short-term instability in attachment quality. Those with high
long-term stability usually come from middle-SES homes with stable family
lives.
5. Cross-cultural evidence
indicates that attachment patterns may have to be interpreted differently
in other culture, but the secure
attachment pattern is the most common in all societies studied.
6. In order for adequate attachment to occur, babies must have
opportunities for attachment to
develop: In a series of studies,
Spitz observed that institutionalized infants experienced emotional
difficulties, wept and withdrew from their surroundings, lost weight, and
had difficulty sleeping. These problems occured not because they were
separated from their mothers, but because they were prevented from forming
a bond with one or a few adults.It is possible that fully normal
attachment development depends on establishing close bonds with caregivers
during the first few years of life.
7. Research findings indicate that
securely attached infants have mothers who engage in sensitive caregivlng—responding
promptly to infant signals, expressing positive emotion, and handling
their babies tenderly and carefully. Insecurely attached infants have
mothers who dislike physical contact, handle them awkwardly, and behave in
a routine” manner when meeting the baby’s needs.
8. Interactional synchrony contributes to the development of secure
attachment. Interactional synchrony is best described as a sensitively
tuned “emotional dance,” in which the caregiver responds to infant
signals in a well-timed, appropriate fashion and both partners match
emotional states, especially the positive ones.
9. Child abuse and neglect are associated with all three forms of insecure
attachment. When caregiving is highly inadequate, it is a powerful
predictor of disruptions in attachment.
Some
of the characteristics that contribute to problems of attachment:
-
Prematurity,
birth complications, and newborn illness are linked to attachment
insecurity in poverty-stricken, stressed families.
The role that temperament plays in attachment security has been
debated. Some evidence indicates that sensitive caregiving can
override the impact of infant characteristics on attachment security.
A major reason that temperament and other infant characteristics do
not show strong relationships with attachment security may be that
their influence depends on goodness-of-fit.
-
In
families where there is stress and instability, insecure attachment is
especially high. Availability of social supports to stressed families
reduces stress and fosters attachment security.
-
Parents
bring to the family context a long history of attachment experiences,
out of which they construct internal working models that they apply to
the bonds established with their babies. Mothers who show objectivity
and balance in discussing their childhoods tend to have securely
attached infants. Mothers who dismiss the importance of early
relationships or describe them in angry, confused ways usually have
insecurely attached babies. This does not mean that negative early
rearing experiences destine us to become insensitive parents.
Internal working models or those early experiences are reconstructed
memories affected by many factors over the life course.
Multiple Attachments and other relationships:
-
Bowlby
believed that infants are predisposed to direct their attachment
behaviors to a single attachment figure and that this preference
typically declines over the second year of life.
-
Fathers’
sensitive caregiving predicts secure attachment—an effect that
becomes stronger the more time they spend with their babies. Usually,
mothers spend more time in physical care, while fathers spend more
time in playful interaction. As a result, babies tend to look to their
mothers when distressed and to their fathers for playful stimulation.This
picture of mother as caregiver and father as playmate has changed in
some families due to the revised work status of women. Highly
involved fathers are less gender stereotyped in their beliefs, have
sympathetic, friendly personalities, and regard parenthood as an
especially enriching experience. They become more similar to mothers
in their parenting style except that they tend to continue to play
more physically actively with their infants.
-
Siblings: 80 percent of American children grow up with at least
one sibling. Conflict between
siblings increases when one member of a sibling pair is emotionally
intense or highly active. Secure infant—mother attachment and warmth
toward both children are related to positive sibling interaction,
whereas coldness is associated with sibling friction. Setting
aside special times and more 'grown-up' activities for the older child
supports sibling harmony.
-
Between
1 and 2 years, coordinated peer interaction occurs more often,
typically in the form of mutual physical play and imitation.
Reciprocal play and positive emotion are especially frequent in
toddlers’ interactions with familiar agemates, suggesting that they
are building true peer relationships. Peer
sociability is present in the first two years, and it is fostered by
the early caregiver—child bond.
-
When
infants and toddlers have day care or babysitters, etc, the continuity
of caregiving determines whether attachment problems arise.
Infants who lack adequate parenting can develop compensating
affectional ties outside the immediate family and can be insulated
from or bounce back from adversity.
-
Quality
of attachment to the main caretaker in infancy is related to cognitive
and social development in early childhood. A child whose parental
caregiving improves can recover and develop normally if the condition
has not lasted too long.
E.
SELF-DEVELOPMENT (PP. 283-286)
Self-Awareness: The earliest aspect of self awareness to
emerge is the I-self—the sense of self as subject, or agent, who
is separate from, but acts on, other objects and other people. During the
second year, toddlers construct the me-self—a reflective observer that
considers the self an object of knowledge and evaluation. Conscious
awareness of the self’s features accompanies development of the me-self.
-
Self-awareness
leads to the child’s initial efforts to understand another’s
perspective.Self-awareness is accompanied by empathy—the ability to
understand and respond sympathetically to the feelings of others.
-
Development
of the me-self permits toddlers to compare themselves to other people.
Between 18 and 30 months children categorize themselves and
others on the basis of age, sex, and even goodness and badness.
-
Self-control: One critical
aspect of development that is very much a part of Erikson's stage of
'autonomy vs. shame and doubt' is the toddlers attempts to begin to
control himself. Self-control is the
capacity to resist an impulse to engage in socially disapproved behavior.
-
The
first signs of self-control appear as compliance—voluntary obedience
to adult requests and commands.
-
Toddlers
who experience positive caregiving and reasonable expectations are
more likely to be compliant and cooperative than oppositional.
-
Toddlers’
control over their own actions is dependent upon parental guidance and
reminders
|