AMOK  CULTURALLY in MALAYSIA

 

                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                              Home Page

 

 

The Amok term in Malaysia and the regions, seems to be more of a ‘concept’ than a concrete state of affairs. This essay finds that there are two interpretations of the phenomenon in the historical record. The phenomenon has several epidemiological tangents, they are martial amok –an act committed in a battle; beramok –a latent form of amok, and true or solitary amok -this is the modern interpretation. Amok is committed by males only; whereas women retain a phenomenon called latah, this is a condition in both men and women and while it involves adverse conscious behaviour, it is not the same as amok. This indicates that there is a cultural aspect to the phenomenon, which may have its origins in antiquity. The aim of this essay is to compare the Malay and the European interpretations of amok in a way that will decisively display the cultural differences. It will then be possible to conclude that for about five-hundred years in Malayan history, amok has been culture bound. The view is that the interpretation can remain in epidemiological historiography. Which culture, and what the interpretations are will become apparent. The classification generally known to be a single act of amok will be the focus of this discussion, and the conclusion will be that it is indeed a culture-bound syndrome.

Firstly, it is important to give a general explanation of the phenomenon of amok. It is an action a person takes, the action being no less than a homicidal attack upon another person or persons. Some ways both Malays and Europeans describe the amok assault (or the factors attached to it) include a depressed state, ‘having seen everything red’, amnesia after the event, grief, sickness of the heart, and suicidal disposition. Also indiscriminate attack, unrestrained violence, drug violence and various other descriptive accounts of homicide. The action -as perceived by others- is seemingly unprovoked, carried out by Malay men and often ending in more than one death as well as the death of the pengamuk; ‘one who goes amok’, Winzeler.

Around the sixteenth century period, the first recorded mentions of amok were to be found, says Spore; he cites Portuguese Journals, as does Winzeler. Whilst the perception of amok from then until the eighteen-hundreds is perhaps somewhat unclear, the indications are that martial amok and solitary amok were the most notable and thus the most discussed and accounted for in the Malay regional history. Spore quotes Crawfurd in capturing the early incidences: ‘the natives of the Eastern islands ran a-muck before they ever heard of the Muhammadan religion, and the unconverted natives at the present day equally run a-muck with the converted’. This evidence was recorded toward the end of the eighteenth century by Crawfurd.

Key evidence for allegations that amok may in fact be cultural is the significance of other peoples not engaging in an action that would be definable as amok. The Chinese in Malay history for example have been noticed for not running amok. Spores claims that no Europeans or Indians have been noted for an amok, however he did find evidence of three Chinese who did go amok; this he claims was during the ‘entire Malay setting’, presumably four hundred and fifty years or more. While Winzeler  says none were to be found, he does offer some explanation for this apparent lack of evidence. The suggestion is that the definition and criteria used to define an attack was different, he also makes the claim that the phenomenon was specific to the Malay and the Dayak of Sarawak. The evidence suggests the culture of the amok phenomenon was present in the Philippines, Sumatra, Java and the ‘margins of the Malayan world’. Whilst these examples are discussions of the ‘culture-bound’ part of the historic record, there is a place for epidemiological or pathological explanation of the syndrome.

There are many suggestions and theories that the epidemiology, the medical interpretation, has changed. For instance, Winzeler suggests that the pathology had changed from a cultural concept to one of mosquito borne illness or drug-related action and on to a psychiatric illness. This alteration in diagnoses is consistent with the evolution of medical and cultural knowledge. In a later effort to divulge the phenomenon, Islam is used as a concept to explain it. Again, the evidence suggests this is a common reasoning and that it extended back to the seventeenth century:- Winzeler, Spores. Winzeler cites Judge Norris of 1842 as having said ‘Mahommedan crimes’ explain amok. In that period of Malay history, it may be likely that the traits of Islam were seen as a motive and this was probably broadly accepted. As Spores indicates, Islam is the dominant religion, and with its principles being open to radical interpretation such as Jihad, the possibility exists that the culture of Islam is partly responsible for amok.

However, the evidence suggests that amok has undergone a metamorphosis from an unremarkable cultural act to a phenomenon shrouded in a mystique that outside cultures do not understand. Winzeler explains Malay people did not necessarily find the practice unusual; that it had ‘heroic history’ and was quite complex and accompanied, on occasion, with some form of motivating humiliation; thus making it not dishonourable within the host culture.

He points out (in Winzeler) that a culture study by Teoh suggests that previously ‘Chinese and Indians only rarely ran amok, it became very common for them to do so – more common, in fact, for the Chinese than the Malays’. Again citing Teoh, Winzeler relates three changes in the evolution of amok: the suggestion is that in the nineteenth century it was only Malays who amoked. Other peoples became amokers in the twentieth century, and that -as well- the causal factors that were held responsible for the act had changed. What this means is that while over the five-hundred year colonial period there had been many cultural, physical environment and social changes, the phenomenon of amok had not changed -people still ‘run amok’.

Yan describes a pathology that may be closer to the truth about amok than what European cultures are prepared to acknowledge. His suggestion is that the homicidal amok is revenge for state sanctioned distress and an escape from oppression into death. And that the host culture realised a necessity to disassociate consciousness from the criteria to avoid blame being sourced to it. This is how the historically tolerant Malay people seem to have constructed amok in its present form. Coupled with the permissive doctrine of Islamic intent toward “infidels”, the act had seemingly become culture-bound. But Yan makes it quite clear that in this instance, as yet, no clear criteria exists that can define a cultural syndrome; this leaves two possibilities in his discussion, is the syndrome universal or unique in cultures.

Yan quotes Gullick of 1958 realising the significance of cultural oppression: ‘amok was also culturally sanctioned as an instrument of social protest against their ruler if he abused his power’. This is a clear indicator that the phenomenon is a social construction, and therefore bound up in Malay cultural history. However, it appears that the European interpretation of amok is necessarily different because it needs to place contextual meaning upon a social phenomenon that may have its criteria constructed by oppression. As a causal factor Crawfurd in Prince established: ‘the spirit of revenge, with an impatience of restraint, and a repugnance to submit to insult’, in Malay culture.

 McKenzie in introducing the subject of culture-bound syndromes, announces: ‘culture changes what sort of delusions and hallucinations are thought to be normal and what are symptoms of madness’. However, the combined evidence’s that are presented to support the various epidemiologies, and the cultural explanations that are presented from the early colonial periods about amok, later find themselves to be argued against and refuted throughout the centuries. Both with emperical evidence and with modern diagnoses of physical conditions that have in the past been applied as a causal factor. For instance, Saint Martin credits Captain Cook in 1770 with the first discovery of amok; and the causal factor was possession by an evil tiger spirit. He is sure: ‘the behaviour observed in amok 200 years ago in the primitive tribes will necessarily differ from that seen in contemporary cases’. Yet the Portuguese evidence recorded homicidal amok in Malayan history in the sixteenth century.

Not tolerating twentieth century diagnoses, Winzeler lambasts Fitzgerald and Galloway, accusing them of bigotry for their logical diagnoses of amok that concluded: ‘defective character…neurotic nature…oppressed condition’; and so on. This effort by Winzeler is a strange claim to make, because his argument is itself an expression of European bigotry.

Prince in his argument quotes Murphy who realises that homicidal assault is a plain occurrence in society and that it occurs all over the world, not just Malaysia. He makes a logical statement: ‘their manifestation, sequels and etiology are so varied that there is little point considering them as comprising a single syndrome’. Interestingly, Prince has used the history of the homicidal Nazis during World War Two in an apparent attempt to compare amok with the horrors of war. However, he fails to acknowledge the methodology of the Gestapo and their Fascist intellectual inquiries in the prelude to war. German society was rife with fascism and internalised oppression. The historical significance of this fact is not realised today; did Germany suffer mass martial amok without Prince realising it in his work on the cultural aspects of amok.

Murphy’s work on the cultural aspects in Hammer confides that: ‘In regard to the usefulness of the general approach the answer is…Once we cease to regard psychiatric syndromes as stable patterns to be found in all societies and epochs, it becomes quite easy to spot apparent shifts’; and ‘Although some of these will undoubtedly prove to have been no more than shifts…I believe that some will prove to represent genuine changes in symptomatology’.

The evidence of the historical record, and the researchers mentioned in this essay suggest that the history and of amok is convoluted and consistently unclear, but that it is a social construction. Amok does occur and that there are two socio-cutural versions of it; one European and the other Malay. The conclusion is that the concept of amok is culture bound as a phenomenon. The bulk of the evidence suggests that it can be viewed as culture bound, and that this can remain intact as there are over thirty-four syndromes listed as endemic in particular cultures. In addition, those early categorisations in history following through to today constitute a useful medical record of syndromes.

 

 

 

 

Home Page

 

 

 

 

 

BIBLIOGRAPHY

 

 

HAMMER, K,. K, Salzinger & S, Sutton. Eds. 1972. Psychopathology:

Contributions from the Social, Behavioural and Biological Sciences. New York: Wiley.

 

KARIM, Wazir, Jahan,. Ed.1990. Emotions of culture. A Malay

Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.

 

McKENZIE, Kwame,. Essentials of psychiatry--4. World psychiatry.

Student BMJ. Vol-5, February 1997.

 

PRINCE, Richard,. AMOK then and now. in:

Transcultural Psychiatric Research Review. Vol-28.1991.

Pp219-229.

 

RIMMER, Peter, J,.& Lisa, M, Allen. Eds. 1990. The Underside of

Malaysian History. Pullers, Prostitutes, Plantation Workers…

Singapore: Singapore University Press.

 

SAINT MARTIN, Manuel, L,. Running Amok: a Modern Perspective on

a Culture Bound Syndrome. in: Primary Care Companion to the

Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. Vol-1, No-3. June 1999.

 

SPORES, John, C,.1998. RUNNING AMOK An Historical Inquiry.

Ohio: Ohio University.

 

TMH: Glossary of Culture-Bound Syndromes. May 2003.

http://weber.uscd.edu/~thall/cbs_glos.html