 |
 |
 |
Otto Loewi – Nobel Lecture
Nobel Lecture, December 12, 1936
The chemical transmission of nerve action
Natural or artificial stimulation of nerves
gives rise to a process of progressive excitation in them, leading
to a response in the effector organ of the nerves concerned.
Up until the year 1921 it was not known how the
stimulation of a nerve influenced the effector organ's function, in
other words, in what way the stimulation was transmitted to the
effector organ from the nerve-ending. In general it was thought that
it came about through direct transmission of the stimulation wave
from the nerve fibre to the effector organ. But the possibility of
transmission by chemical means had also been considered and
experiments had been conducted on these lines. As a result of his
own experiments, Howell1
had come to believe that vagus stimulation released potassium in the
heart and that this was the cause of the resultant effect, and
Bayliss2
discussed the possibility, in view of the similarity in action of
the so-called vagomimetic substances and chorda stimulation, that
this stimulation might be caused by the production of such
substances. Although these data were known to me, my attention was
only drawn years after my discovery to the fact that earlier (in
1904 to be exact) Elliott3,
in the last paragraph of a short note, suggested the possibility
that the stimulation of sympathetic nerves might be brought about by
the release of adrenaline, and that Dixon4
had already communicated experiments in an inaccessible site to test
whether, during vagus stimulation, a substance was released which
contributed to the stimulation reaction.
In the year 1921 I was successful for the first
times in obtaining certain proof that by stimulation of the nerves
in a frog's heart substances were released which to some extent
passed into the heart fluid and, when transferred with this into a
test heart, caused it to react in exactly the same way as the
stimulation of the corresponding nerves. In this way it was proved
that the nerves do not act directly upon the heart, but rather that
the direct result of nerve stimulation is the release of chemical
substances and that it is these which bring directly about
characteristic changes of function in the heart.
It was, of course, possible right from the start
that this mechanism which I described at the time as "humoral
transference", but which is now known as "chemical" transference as
the result of a well-founded suggestion by H.H. Dale, does not
represent an isolated phenomenon but a special condition which also
appears elsewhere. We shall soon see that this supposition was
justified. But before I go into that I should like to characterize
in more detail the substances which are released by nerve
stimulation and produce the effect. First of all, I must mention my
distinguished collaborators E. Navratil, W. Witanowski, and E.
Engelhart, and thank them.
Let me begin with the transfer medium of the
reaction in vagus stimulation which I have called "vagus substance".
We were able to determine that its effect is inhibited by atropine5
and very quickly disappear6.
In looking for a substance with both these characteristics, I found
that out of a series of the known vagomimetic substances, muscarine,
piloearpin, choline, and acetylcholine, only the last-named
possessed them7.
We were then able to establish further that the rapid disappearance
of the action of the vagus substance and acetylcholine (Ac.Ch.)
through the breaking down of these substances was caused by the
action of an esterase in the heart6,
which had already been postulated by Dale8.
I was able to show furthermore that the action of this esterase
could be specifically inhibited through minimum concentrations of
eserine7.
This discovery was important not only because, for the first time,
the operational mechanism of an alkaloid had been revealed, but
especially because the discovery enabled the theory of the chemical
transference of nerve stimulation to be developed for the first
time. On the one hand, this eserine action provided a means of
revealing the minimal quantities of Ac.Ch. being released by nerve
stimulation which would otherwise, because of their rapid
destructibility, have remained undisclosed. On the other hand, we
are able, in cases where for any reason it is technically impossible
or difficult to prove directly the release of Ac.Ch. in nerve
stimulation, to draw the conclusion indirectly from the increase in
effect of nerve stimulation after previous eserination that the
nerve stimulation is being produced by the release of Ac.Ch. And now
we must return to the characterization of the vagus substance.
The vagus substance behaves identically with
Ac.Ch. not only in regard to its reaction to atropine, and to its
destructibility with esterase but also concerning all other
characteristics. As Dale and Dudley9
were able to produce it directly from the organs, there can be no
more doubt that the vagus substance is Ac.Ch. and in future I shall
refer to it as such.
As regards the character of the substance which
is released through stimulation of the sympathetic nerves of the
heart and other organs, I was able to show earlier that it shares
many properties with adrenaline; both, for example, are destroyed by
alkali20
and by fluorescence and ultraviolet light6,
the activity of both is abolished by ergotamine21;
on the other hand, as Cannon and Rosenblueth10
have shown, it is raised by small and in themselves ineffective
quantities of cocaine, the adrenaline-sensitizing action of which
Fröhlich and I11
found some 25 years ago.
Like the effect of adrenaline, an equal
effective strength of the sympathicus substance declines very slowly
in the heart, much more slowly incidentally than might have been
expected in view of the rapid oxidizability of adrenaline or
sympathicus substance in vitro. The cause of this, as revealed by
Dr. Ralph Smith of Ann Arbor and me in a series of specially
conducted experiments not as yet published, turns out to be the
giving off of substances from the heart which inhibit adrenaline
oxidation. There must, of course, be some physiological purpose in
the fact that individual devices exist, on the one hand to remove
the acetylcholine as quickly as possible and the adrenaline, on the
other hand, as slowly as possible. And now we must return to the
chemical nature of the sympathicus substance.
Although for some time it had been considered
probable after all we had seen that the sympathicus substance was
adrenaline, I was only able to give direct proof of it this year.
Gaddum and Schild13,
on the basis of a statement by Paget, investigated the significance
of a green fluorescence visible in ultraviolet light which pointed
to adrenaline in the presence of O2, and alkali, and
found that this appears to a high degree specific for adrenaline. I
was now able to show that not only the heart extract, but also the
heart fluid, shows this reaction after accelerated periods of
stimulation12.
Accordingly I consider it proved that the sympathicus substance is
adrenaline.
Now I must briefly consider the question of to
what extent the neuro-chemical mechanism, that is to say the
chemical transference of nerve stimulation, is important other than
to the heart.
Firstly, Rylant14
and others were able to show that with warm-blooded animals too,
vagus stimulation released Ac.Ch. which was responsible for the
resultant stimulation reaction. I must mention in this connection
that my collaborator Engelhart15
was able to show, in accordance with the well-known fact that the
heart vagus in warm-blooded animals ends at the auricular/
ventricular boundary, that here considerably more Ac.Ch. was to be
found before and after stimulation in the auricle than in the
ventricle, whereas in a frog's heart, where the vagus extends over
the ventricle as well, the distribution of Ac.Ch. over auricle and
ventricle is even. As the heart vagus belongs to the parasympathetic
system, the question had to be examined whether and to what extent
the neurochemical mechanism applied here. The first investigation on
this point also came from my Institute, from Engelhart16
, who was able to prove the release of Ac.Ch. as a result of
stimulation of the oculomotor nerve. The total result of the many
different, resultant investigations on various organs can be
summarized by saying that up until now no single case is known in
which the effect of the stimulation of the parasympathetic nerves
was not caused by the release of Ac.Ch.
As, to my mind, a lecture should concern itself
not only with results, but also with still open questions, I must
touch on the following: As all activity caused by the application of
Ac.Ch. can be halted by atropine, one might expect that wherever
Ac.Ch. is released as a result of nerve stimulation, the effect
could everywhere be halted by atropine. This, however, is not so.
Contractions of the bladder after stimulation of the pelvic nerve,
dilation of the vessels of the salivary gland after stimulation of
the chorda nerve still occur even after atropinization. And here we
must mention the following strange observation by V.E.
Henderson17:
he found that after preliminary atropinization, vagus stimulation in
the intestine produced no increase of tonus, but an increase of
peristaltic contractions. The reason for these remarkable exceptions
has so far escaped us.
The neurochemical mechanism is everywhere
apparent in the field of activity of the parasympathetic system, as
in the sympathetic system. But we have Dale18
and his collaborators to thank for the recognition that the
stimulation of certain nerve fibres which belong anatomically to the
sympathetic system lead to the release, not of adrenaline, as in the
overwhelmingly large number of cases, but of Ac.Ch.
To sum up then, it may be said that the
neurochemical mechanism applies in the stimulation of all autonomic
nerves.
But it also embraces a much wider area. We owe
this knowledge in the main to the basic investigations of Dale.
There is no need, therefore, for me to go further into this in my
lecture.
We now have to discuss the important question of
whether the nerve stimulation influences only the function of the
effector organ by the release of nerve substances, as I will call
the chemical transmitters for the sake of briefness, or whether it
perhaps exerts another influence as well.
Here we shall be well advised to take as a
starting-point the mechanism of action of atropine or ergotamine.
With Navratil19
I was able to show (and this finding was confirmed many times over)
that these alkaloids do not, as had been thought previously, attack
and incapacitate the nerves themselves. We were able to show this by
demonstrating that even after using atropine and ergotamine, nerve
stimulation still released nerve substances. This shows that
atropine and ergotamine do not impair the function of the nerves,
which is a liberating one, that is to say, they do not paralyse the
nerves, but exert an antagonistic influence on the action of the
substances produced. By recognizing that after previous application
of atropine or ergotamine the stimulation of the respective nerves
is known to have no effect at all upon the effector organ, it has
been proved that nerve stimulation has no other effect but to
release nerve substances. What other kind of function can remain for
the nerve if the action of the substance released coincides
absolutely with the effect of the nerve stimulation? Although what
follows is self-explanatory, I still think it desirable to state it
expressly: in all cases in which the neurochemical mechanism occurs,
the nerves only control function to the extent of the release of the
substance: the place where this occurs is in the effector organ of
the nerve. From then onwards, the released substance exerts control:
the functioning organ is, therefore, its effector organ
exclusively.
And now we must consider in which directions our
knowledge of the physiological process has been extended, beyond
what we have already said, by the discovery of the neurochemical
mechanism.
There will be no cause for argument if we see
the most importance in the fact that at last a clear answer has been
found to the age-old question as to the nature of the
stimulus-transfer from nerve to effector organ.
Next in importance appears to me to be the
explanation of the nature of the peripheral inhibition. Up until
now, it appeared quite inconceivable that the stimulation of a nerve
could lead to inhibition in the effector organ. With the proof that
this inhibition comes about because the nerve releases a
function-inhibiting substance, the reason for it becomes clear. At
the same time, however, something else is proved which seems to me
to be of great importance: the release of a substance by the nerves
is the expression of a positive function, an activation. This proves
that the direct effect of the stimulation of all nerves,
whether activating or inhibitory, represents a promotion of
function, for this is what the release of the substance does.
Today, because we know how it happens, this
solution strikes us as self-evident. For, since the process of
stimulation is, to a certain degree, unspecific and furthermore
interference in stimulus frequencies which certainly form the basis
of some inhibitory manifestations in the animal region of the
central nervous system cannot, in the case of peripherally
inhibitable organs, be regarded as the cause of inhibition, I see no
other possibility, at least in general, as to how nerve stimulation
can lead to inhibitions of the effector organ at all than by
chemical means; in other words, the chemical mechanism is the only
conceivable way.
So much for the field of activity and the
importance of the neurochemical mechanism.
After this description which touches upon the
general nature only of the neurochemical mechanism, we will now
consider more exactly its finer mechanism.
First of all the question arises: where are the
substances released by nerve stimulation localized, or, in other
words, where is the point of attack of the nerve stimulation? A
priori, two possibilities exist: the substances are released in the
nerve endings or in the effector organ. Investigations of this
question carried out so far are concerned only with Ac.Ch.
For the time being we shall only draw upon
findings which concern the Ac.Ch. content of organs after nerve
degeneration.
As far back as 30 years ago, Anderson22
observed the following: after degenerative division of the
oculomotor nerve, light stimulation was for a long time without
effect, regardless of whether the eye had been eserinized or not.
There followed a period when light stimulus was still ineffective to
the uneserinized eye, but not to the eserinized eye. At this moment,
as could be shown, a weak regeneration of the oculomotor nerve had
begun. In Anderson's time it was not possible to give an adequate
explanation of these findings. Today, when we know that oculomotor
stimulation releases AC.Ch., the action of eserine is revealed as
being simply to increase the effect of the Ac.Ch. by inhibiting that
of the esterase, and Anderson's results become absolutely clear.
With degeneration of the oculomotor nerve the Ac.Ch. disappears.
Eserine then also becomes ineffective. With the start of
regeneration of the oculomotor nerve the Ac.Ch. appears again, but
in too small quantities to cause miosis with light stimulus alone,
i.e. without the increased activity provided by eserine. Thus
Anderson's experiments provide the first proof that the existence of
Ac.Ch. in the eye is dependent upon the nerves. Later Engelhart16
in my own Institute produced this proof in a direct manner. With
direct Ac.Ch. determination he found that after degeneration of the
oculomotor nerve in corpus ciliare and iris, the Ac.Ch., present in
considerable quantities in preserved nerves, completely disappears.
This shows that, in many organs at any rate, the Ac.Ch. content and
its maintenance is connected with the presence of the nerve. There
are two possible explanations for the disappearance of the Ac.Ch.
after nerve degeneration. Either the Ac.Ch. is a part of the nerve
and disappears then naturally with its degeneration, or it belongs
to the effector organ. Then we should have to assume that the
formation and maintenance of the Ac.Ch. amount in the effector organ
was, in some mysterious and trophic manner, dependent upon the
nerve, so that it would disappear with its degeneration. Should the
Ac.Ch. be a product of the effector organ and not the nerve ending,
then, according to Dale, it would have to disappear, after
degeneration, through some kind of atrophy. This hypothesis would
then require a further subhypothesis, that of separate and specific
transmission system in the effector organ quite unlike any other.
This assumption would be necessary, because, after oculomotor nerve
degeneration, the effector organs, corpus ciliare and iris do not
degenerate, and yet the Ac.Ch. disappears. The influence of the
oculomotor nerve degeneration must, in that case, only extend to the
mysterious transmission system. In respect of these difficulties
alone, a far likelier assumption is that the Ac.Ch. which is
released by nerve stimulation belongs to the neurone itself, or more
exactly to the nerve ending. There is in my opinion, in at least one
instance, compelling proof for the correctness of this
supposition.
In Dale's Institute, Feldberg and Gaddum23
have shown that stimulation of the preganglionic sympathetic fibres
in the neck releases Ac.Ch. in the sup.cerv. ganglion, which itself
stimulates the ganglion, so that progressive stimulation is set up
in the postganglionic fibres. In elegant experiments directed
towards the question of the localization of the release of Ac.Ch. in
the ganglion, Feldberg and Vartiainen24
were recently able to prove that it was released neither by the
preganglionic fibres nor by the ganglion cells themselves, the only
direct effector organ. They concluded, therefore, that the Ac.Ch.
was produced in the synapse. Synapse is not an anatomical but a
purely functional concept. It indicates the spot where the nerve
ending comes into contact with the cell, and has been adopted by
histologists only in this sense. If, therefore, it can be proved
that Ac.Ch. is formed in the "synapse", it can only, in my opinion,
be in the preganglionic nerve ending or in the ganglion cell. As the
ganglion cell can be ruled out, as Feldberg and Vartiainen have
shown, there only remains, it appears to me, the nerve ending as the
site of release. Although proof of this has so far only been
obtained directly in the case of preganglionic sympathetic endings,
there is, nevertheless, much to make us think that in other places
as well the nerve substances are released in the nerve endings
themselves. We know that in many organs by no means each single,
functioning unit is accorded a nerve fibre. At most, according to
Stöhr, one occurs for every hundred capillaries. When the nerve is
stimulated, however, all react. In these cases, how does the nerve
substance diffuse to those regions without nerves? I believe that
the nerve ending is here the liberation centre. This supposition is
supported when we consider that when the autonomous nerves are
stimulated the two same substances are always released in very
different organs having a quite different chemical structure and
accordingly undergoing quite different chemical changes. If the
substances were not being released in the nerve endings, but
peripherally of them, then we should again have to assume the
presence of some mysterious mechanism capable of transferring the
stimulation of the nerve ending to the supposed peripheral position
where the substance would be released; in which case, the discovery
of the neurochemicalmechanism would not, in my opinion, represent
any important progress.
We come now to the next question concerning this
delicate mechanism.
So far we have only spoken of the release of the
substance from the nerve ending. This is only to say that a free
nerve substance emerges from the nerve ending. But it is important
for an understanding of the nature of nerve function to know what
exactly we should imagine is implied by this release. A priori the
following possibilities exist: either the substances are not present
in the nerve ending when the nerves are in a state of rest and are
only formed by nerve stimulation and, once formed, diffuse, or they
are already present in the state of rest, but can only diffuse after
stimulation. As regards the formation of nerve substances through
the nerves, it is certain that this can be done. Even Witanowski25
in his day found Ac.Ch. in the vagus, in the sympathicus and in the
sympathetic ganglia. The last two findings were confirmed by Chang
and Gaddum.26
As Ac.Ch. is not present in the blood, it cannot diffuse from there,
and neither, on account of its ready destructibility, could it
diffuse from elsewhere in the nerves and ganglia. The same applies
for adrenaline. Recently we have succeeded in showing the presence
of adrenaline in a frog's brain in a state of rest or even
anaesthetized, and also in the upper cervical ganglia of cattle. It
was characterized by its effect upon the heart which was similar to
that of adrenaline, through the neutralizing of this effect by
ergotaminization and also by its destructibility through fluorescent
light. These findings, therefore, confirm that the nerve substances
are formed by the nerve and are present even in a state of rest.
Whether the nerve, when stimulated, produces further substance as
well is another still undecided question which we are not touching
upon here. However interesting in itself the answer to this question
may be, it does not appear to me to be of essential importance,
since the basic effect of nerve stimulation is the release of the
substances. There are two possibilities as regards the processes of
release and diffusion: either the substances are present in a free
and diffusible state in the nerve ending, but the nerve ending when
in a state of rest is impermeable and only made permeable to them
after stimulation, when they become diffusible and effective, or,
the substances in the resting state are in some way combined and
indiffusible and only the stimulation releases the combination and
thereby makes them diffusible and effective. If the first
possibility were to apply, then we must not find the Ac.Ch. at all,
since, as has been shown, esterase is found everywhere in the nerves
and this, as we shall soon see, destroys the free Ac.Ch. But we do
find it in the nerve. This fact alone suffices to show that it is
not present in a free, diffusible state in the nerve ending. In
addition, Bergami28
recently found, in confirmation of earlier experiments by Calabro27,
that Ac.Ch. only issues from the free end of severed nerves if the
nerve is stimulated. In this case, the release cannot, of course, be
attributed to any change in the state of permeability brought about
by stimulation, since the free nerve ending has no membrane. The
second possibility which I mentioned earlier must apply, namely that
the Ac.Ch. in the unstimulated nerves is bound in some way and
thereby protected from the assault of the esterase. In fact, it is
present in such quantity in hearts where there is no vagus
stimulation, that in a freely diffusible state it would be more than
sufficient to stop the heart altogether. On its own it is
ineffectual and is protected against the action of the esterase, in
contrast to when it is in a diffusible state.
In experiments directed towards the study of
this question Engelhart and I29
found the following: If one determines the initial value of Ac.Ch.
in a heart section, leaving the remaining portion of the heart
intact for a few hours, as much Ac.Ch. is found in it afterwards as
in the beginning. Dale and Dudley, incidentally, found the same in
the case of the spleen. In an organ in a state of rest, therefore,
the Ac.Ch. is protected against the esterase. But if free (that is
to say diffusible) Ac.Ch. is added to a heart in a state of rest, it
is destroyed. All this goes to show that obviously, as Dale also
assumed, the Ac.Ch. is present in the organ in a state of rest in
some kind of loose, non-diffusible combination, and for that reason
it is non-susceptible to attack by esterase and non-effective. Such
combinations we know do very often occur in an organism. The
so-called "vehicle function" of the blood implies in fact no more
than the ability of the blood's component parts to bind substances
and, when necessary, to release them. But the binding must in any
case be a very loose one, as after destroying the structure, for
instance by mincing the organ, the Ac.Ch. is very quickly destroyed
by esterase. Nerve stimulation would accordingly appear to have the
effect of releasing from this combination the Ac.Ch. which has been
proved to be present in the nerve.
The same applies also for the nerve substance in
the sympathetic system, adrenaline. As I was able to show this
year12,
the heart contains 1 gamma to 2 gamma per gram, which corresponds to
a concentration of 1:1 million to 1:500,000. Whereas adrenaline
added to the heart will already be effective in a concentration of
1:100 million to the maximum, the concentration of 100-200 times
more adrenaline in a heart in a state of rest will be without
effect. Therefore it also must be present in some kind of inactive
combination in the heart. This fact also seems to me to be of
importance in the possible interpretation of certain other findings.
It is known that in many organs the adrenaline action is quickly
over. Up until now this has been explained by the speedy oxidation
of adrenaline. This is certainly the case for pure adrenaline
solutions in vitro. In vivo, on the other hand,
adrenaline is not only not easily oxidized, but all the organs
contain substances - among them, as has been proved, amino acids -
which, even in minimal quantities, have a direct inhibiting effect
upon the oxidation of adrenaline. How then does this rapid cessation
of activity come about? It may, in part, be due to counteractions.
In some cases, however, the disappearance of activity could be due
to rapid transference of the adrenaline into an ineffective linkage
as is to be found in the heart.
Now let us return from this digression to the
subject of the release of the nerve substances. This occurs very
quickly and the action of the released nerve substance is very rapid
also, although between release and effect the diffusion process has
also to be set in motion. The time interval varies in length in
different cases, but is in part certainly dependent upon the
distance of the releasing nerve ending from the effector cell.
According to Brown and Eccles30
this is 80-100 omega in the case of the heart, but only 2 omega in
the ganglionic synapse. This must mean that release coincides with
stimulation. Dale is able to explain quite easily the fact that the
effect reaches the ganglion cell almost without any time lapse by
the fact that the release in the nerve ending occurs directly with
contact with the ganglion cell, whereas in the heart, where
incidentally the first contraction after vagus stimulation is
smaller, a certain time is required for diffusion to the effector
cells. As in the case of release and effect, the speed with which
the substance and with it the effect disappears, varies in different
objects. The discovery of the chemical mechanism of the effect of
vagus stimulation in the heart was only possible because in this
case the destruction of the Ac.Ch. occurs so slowly that the
substance had time to diffuse, in sufficient quantity to be active,
into the heart; in the ganglia on the other hand, the destruction
occurs so rapidly that the Ac.Ch. in the perfusion fluid is only
demonstrable after preliminary eserination. The differences in time
between freeing and disappearance in both cases are easily
understandable if we consider the quite different purposes which the
nerve stimulation serves in both these cases.
And now, finally, we come to the localization of
the point of attack of the nerve substances.
As long as it was not known that the autonomic
nerves, when stimulated, release substances which condition the
successful effect of the nerve stimulation, it was assumed in
general, in consideration of the fact that the action picture of the
so-called vago- and sympathico-mimetic substances is identical with
the stimulating of the corresponding nerves, and, further, with the
fact that it was believed that the alkaloids, atropine, and
ergotamine, which inhibit the action of the substances, paralyse the
corresponding nerves, that the vago- and sympathico-mimetic
substances stimulate the nerves somewhere peripherally. But as they
are effective even after nerve degeneration, it was assumed, with
justification at the time, that a non-degenerative myoneural
junction was the point of attack. Today, now that we know that the
nerves do release nerve substances, this view is no longer tenable.
The nerve substances, considered as vago- or sympathico-mimetic
substances, would have to act like these, that is to say, they would
have to stimulate the myoneural junction and release substances,
etc. on their own. In this case there would be no kind of effect
upon the effector organ. Quite apart from this, the supposition that
the nerve substances stimulate the nerve somewhere is quite
superfluous by the proof shown above, that the alkaloids atropine
and ergotamine which inhibit the activity of the vago- and
sympathico-mimetic substances, do not, as was supposed, paralyse the
nerves, but are simply antagonistic to the substances. If all this
is evidence against the nerve as point of attack, it has also been
proved that Ac.Ch. and adrenaline are also effective in the absence
of nerves. Ac.Ch., for instance, dilates vessels which are not
parasympathetically innervated. Adrenaline increases the activity of
the still nerveless embryonic heart and stimulates the arrectores
pilorum, which, according to Stöhr, are also nerveless, etc.
Therefore, the point of attack of the nerve substances must be some
part of the effector organ itself, probably chemical or
chemico-physical in character and not morphological.
As Dale has proved, we can no longer say that
the nerve substances reproduce the action picture of the nerves but
rather it is a fact that the nerves reproduce the action picture of
the substances, since they release these and thus lead to effective
action. That the activity caused by any one nerve substance appears
principally at the spot where it is released, that is to say, that
in that particular spot the cells are receptive to its action, is a
local phenomenon of the specific sensitivity to certain chemical
substances which is met with everywhere in the living organism and
which is Erie of the foundations of its function and, therefore, of
its very existence and which can only be understood teleologically
and not causally; think, for example, of the finely graduated,
specific sensitivity of the respiratory centre to
CO2.
Up until now we have discussed only the effect
of the nerve substances on the organ in which they are released
through nerve stimulus. Are they only active there, or in other
distant organs too? We have already mentioned that a part of the
released substance diffuses into the blood or into some other
perfusing fluid. This could present the possibility of its action
being extended to other more distant organs. What is the position
here? Given special conditions, which I would like to characterize
as pathological, this could happen.It has been proved that when the
breaking up of the Ac.Ch. by an esterase, is inhibited by eserine,
the Ac.Ch. penetrates with the blood to other organs in sufficient
quantities to cause activity. Furthermore, Cannon31
by preliminary sensitizing of organs through denervation, or
cocainization, made them so hypersensitive to the sympathicus
substance that they reacted to its release in any organ. In the same
way as in these experimentally induced disturbances, it could also
happen perhaps that in cases of illness, the release of surplus
quantities of substance or incomplete destruction may interrupt the
normal release and destruction, leading to hypersensitivity of
organs and the appearance of effect at a distance. It would be very
desirable if in future clinicians would give consideration to these
relationships with a view to explaining certain symptoms and groups
of symptoms which until now, partly without sufficient foundation,
have been considered as purely reflex. Under normal conditions,
however, the effect of the nerve substance would be limited to the
organ in which it is released. The hormones are there to exert a
general control, that is to say not a localized chemical one, on the
organs.
In conclusion a word or two on the question of
how the neurochemical mechanism fits into the connecting pattern of
cells. With the discovery that its influence comes about through
substances which are released by the nervous system itself, we have
the first proof that the nervous system is not only an effector
organ for chemical influences from outside, and not only a
participant in general metabolism, but that it has itself a specific
chemical influence upon happenings in the organism. On closer
examination this is not surprising.
In nerve-free multicellular organisms, the
relationships of the cells to each other can only be of a chemical
nature. In multicellular organisms with nerve systems, the nerve
cells only represent cells like any others, but they have extensions
suited to the purpose which they serve, namely the nerves.
Accordingly it is perhaps only natural that the relationships
between the nervous system and other organs should be qualitatively
of the same kind as that between the non-nervous organs among
themselves, that is to say, of a chemical nature.
1. W.H. Howell, Am. J.
Physic., 21 (1908) 51.
2. W.M. Bayliss,
Principles of General Physiology, 3rd ed., 1920, p. 344.
3. T.R. Elliott, J.
Physiol., 31 (1904) 20 P.
4. W.E. Dixon, Med.
Mag., 16 (1907) 454.
5. O. Loewi, Pflügers
Arch. Ges. Physiol., 189 (1921) 239.
6. O. Loewi and E.
Navratil, Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 214 (1926) 678.
7. O. Loewi and E.
Navratil, Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 214 (1926) 689.
8. H.H. Dale, J.
Pharmacol., 68 (1924) 107.
9. H.H. Dale and H.W.
Dudley, J. Physiol., 68 (1929) 97:
10. W.B. Cannon and A.
Rosenblueth, Am. J. Physiol., 99 (1932) 392.
11. A. Fröhlich and O.
Loewi, Arch. Exptl. Pathol. Pharmakol., 62 (1910) 159.
12. O. Loewi, Pflügers
Arch. Ges. Physiol., 237 (1936) 504.
13. J.H. Gaddum and H.
Schild, J. Physiol., 80 (1934)9 P.
14. P. Rylant, Compt.
Rend. Soc. Biol., 96 (1927) 1054.
15. E. Engelhart,
Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 225 (1930) 722.
16. E. Engekart,
Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 227 (1931) 220.
17. V.E. Henderson,
Arch. Intern. Pharmacodyn., 27 (1922) 205.
18. H.H. Dale, J.
Physiol., 80 (1933) 10P.
19. O. Loewi and E.
Navratil, Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 206 (1924) 123. E.
Navratil, Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 217 (1927) 610.
20. O. Loewi, Pflügers
Arch. Ges. Physiol., 193 (1921) 201.
21. O. Loewi, Pflügers
Arch. Ges. Physiol., 203 (1924) 408.
22. H.K. Anderson, J.
Physiol., 33 (1905) 156, 414.
23. W. Feldberg and J.H.
Gaddum, J. Physiol., 81 (1934) 305.
24. W. Feldberg and A.
Vartiainen, J. Physiol., 83 (1934) 103.
25. W.R. Witanowski,
Pflügers Arch. Ges. Physiol., 208 (1925) 694.
26. H.C. Chang and J.H.
Gaddum, J. Physiol., 79 (1933) 255.
27. Q. Calabro, Riv.
Biol., 19 ( 1935).
28. G. Bergami, Klin.
Wockschr., 15 (1936) 1030.
29. E. Engelhart and O.
Loewi, Arch. Intern. Pharmacodyn., 38 (1930) 287.
30. G.L. Brown and J.C.
Eccles, J. Physiol., 82 (1934) 211.
31. W.B. Cannon and Z.M.
Bacq, Am. J. Physiol., 96 (1931) 392.
From
Nobel Lectures, Physiology or Medicine 1922-1941.
|
 |
 |
 |