R.S.D. Article # 4

 

Commentary on RSD focus article

Beginning
Whenever two or three pain doctors are gathered together there is a high 
probability that the conversation will drift to reflex sympathetic dystrophy 
(RSD). I think the reasons are that we all see RSD patients, that there is a 
tantalising link between our needle therapies and improvement, and that in 
the absence of evidence opinion holds sway - ideal fertile ground for 
conversation.
 

Tanelian's focus article highlights some of the areas of ignorance:
 

what causes RSD?

what is the natural history?

how best should it be managed?
 

This paper will take these three points briefly and then explore the wider 
ramifications of how we should approach clinical research into this and other 
pain conditions, pursuing Tanelian's digression into placebo.
 

WHAT CAUSES RSD?
The IASP definition of "continuous pain in an extremity after trauma which 
may include fracture but does not involve a major nerve, associated with 
sympathetic hyperactivity" together with its rider is a succinct and 
thoughtful description of the problem. The presence of sympathetic 
hyperactivity can be demonstrated with greater or lesser ease. The problem is 
that the presence of sympathetic hyperactivity has been taken to mean that 
sympathetic hyperactivity causes RSD. This is akin to saying that it is dark 
outside and the street lights are on, therefore the street lights have caused 
the darkness. As a buttress to the argument that sympathetic hyperactivity 
causes RSD it is argued that sympathetic blockade cures RSD, therefore 
sympathetic hyperactivity must have caused the RSD. This is a false syllogism 
because, as Tanelian points out, the evidence that sympathetic blocks cure 
RSD is far from strong. Patients may improve after treatment which includes 
sympathetic blockade, which is splendid, but there is no proof that it is the 
sympathetic block rather than a change from disuse to use of the limb which 
has effected the change.
 

The answer to the question "what causes RSD?" is that we do not know. Why 
don't all patients with a Colles' fracture develop RSD?, or why, if all 
patients have an injury response which could go on to develop into RSD, do 
only some patients present with the syndrome? There is an obvious analogy to 
pains which persist after surgery. A proportion (?1%) of patients develop 
pain after herniorrhaphy. As with RSD we do not know what is different about these particular herniorrhaphy patients, and why they go on to develop 
persistent pain when their peers do not.
 

WHAT IS THE NATURAL HISTORY?
Once more the answer is that we do not know. Tanelian quotes Subbarao and 
Stilwell' audit [Subbarao and Stilwell, 1981] that, fourteen months after 
their last treatment, two-thirds of 125 RSD cases had retired, were 
officially disabled or did not return to the same job. This was despite the 
physicians reporting 51% as having excellent or good response 2.5 months 
after treatment. It suggests strongly that benefit of the treatment did not 
always persist and that this is not always a self-limiting disorder.
 

Tanelian discusses the oft-quoted contention that results of early treatment 
will be better than those when the pain is treated late. If there is an 
element of self-limitation in RSD (the one-third of the 125 cases who had not 
retired or had to change their jobs), then early treatment may claim as cures 
those who would have improved anyhow. The unknown is the extent to which 
early treatment could cure those who would otherwise have had pain of long 
duration. The Subbarao and Stilwell audit does not help to answer this 
question.
 

Our ignorance of the natural history means that any comparison of treatment 
intervention must be organised, for instance by stratified randomisation, so 
that duration of the syndrome among the patients in each treatment group is 
balanced. Testing a magical new cure on 'early' RSD patients and comparing it 
with standard treatment on long-standing RSD patients might well give a 
misleading answer.
 

HOW BEST SHOULD RSD BE MANAGED?
One way to approach this question is to turn it around and ask what 
constitutes a good clinical result, for the patient and for us. The simple 
answer is that function of the limb returns to normal, and that pain 
disappears. There is a catch here - which is the most important outcome, 
restoration of function or disappearance of the pain?, or are both equally 
important? For the design of future treatment comparisons we need to plan the 
outcome measures more carefully, to allow for the fact that we may get 
different answers for function and for pain.
 

Tanelian's focus article provides a huge number of studies of treatments for 
RSD in Table 1. It is not clear whether or not he has looked at the full deck 
of cards ("I have reviewed the majority of studies cited in Medline"). He 
cites reasonable criteria for study validity, five out of the six quantitative diagnosis of RSD, cross-over design, randomisation, prospective, 
double-blind and placebo-controlled. He does not tell us how many of the 
studies in Table 1 meet these criteria, although we could work it out for 
ourselves. Most importantly there is no qualitative or quantitative attempt 
to pull this data together and to give us a clinically useful conclusion. 
Instead he chickens out, and says "Having reviewed the literature, I agree 
with the conclusion Betcher reached in 1953 ... "(that) sympathetic blockade 
is a beneficial means to reduce pain and thereby allow patients to conduct 
physical therapy, and resolve their manifestations of RSD".
 

The process pundits would argue that this is not a valid systematic review 
[Mulrow, 1987]. We are not told how hard the author pushed to obtain his 
citations, or his search strategy or whether or not he pursued citations in 
the reference sections of the articles he identified. There is no judgement 
presented of the quality standards of the articles identified. He has 
included non-randomised studies in the Table despite his mention in the text 
of the fact that non-randomised studies can lead to overestimation of 
therapeutic effect of up to 40% [Schultz et al, 1995]. If this reads like 
harsh criticism it is only because I really want to know the answer, and what 
shines through is that we do not have the randomised studies we need to 
provide such an answer. My concern is that by quoting the non-randomised 
studies we perpetuate the current state of ignorance. Only by admitting that 
ignorance can we move forward, declare a research agenda and set about the 
design and execution of the studies required to fulfil the agenda.
 

The way forward
What we really want to provide is interventions which break the cycle of 
inability to use the limb. We can (to an extent) help with the pain, but we 
want to reverse the disuse if possible. If needle interventions ,such as 
sympathetic blocks, from intravenous regional to stellate to lumbar 
sympathetic, can achieve this aim then that is marvellous. What remains 
unclear is whether sustained reversal is possible, in which patients if not 
all can this be achieved, and what it takes to achieve the reversal. This is 
very close to Tanelian's quote from Betcher "sympathetic blockade is a 
beneficial means to reduce pain and thereby allow patients to conduct 
physical therapy".
 

We do need to be sure that our blocks do not make things worse. We need the 
simplest and safest procedure to achieve our goal. If local anaesthetic can 
achieve the facilitation of physical therapy then we should be using local 
anaesthetic rather than neurolytics, because local anaesthetic is safer. If 
more sustained block is required would local anaesthetic infusion be more 
sensible than neurolytics?
 

DESIGN OF STUDIES
Studies in this field, as in others, need to be randomised to minimise 
selection bias. Ideally they should be double-blind, although this may be 
logistically difficult to organise for needle interventions. They need to be 
of adequate size to have the power to tell us that a negative result is a 
true negative. This would be buttressed by including in the design an active 
control known to work in the condition, so that we would know that the 
investigators could distinguish that active control from either the test 
treatment or a negative control (placebo). Unfortunately in RSD, as in many 
other conditions, there is no clear candidate as standard treatment to act as 
an active control.
 

The outcome measures need to include function as well as pain. Ideally they 
should include a dichotomous clinically intelligible component, like the 
time-honoured "Is your pain half gone?". Follow-up needs to be at least a 
year, judging from the data which Tanelian quotes. 
 

None of this is easy. Tanelian helpfully includes Table 2 and text to 
indicate that the numbers of patients required to answer these questions are 
substantial. Such calculations require making a guess at the placebo 
response. A core problem here is that we do not know the natural history. 
Some of these patients will get better despite the treatment. Tanelian quotes 
a conservative estimate of the placebo response as 35%, while saying that 
many studies report rates which are higher.
 

As part of a series of systematic reviews on neuropathic pain treatment 
[McQuay et al, in press] we have used L'Abbé plots to try to pin down the 
variability in trial results due to just this phenomenon of 'success' (pain 
relief) in the control groups [L'Abbé and Detsky, 1987]. Each trial is a 
point on the scatter plot, using the success rate with the intervention under 
test (Experimental Event Rate, EER; y axis) and the success rate in the 
control group (Control Event Rate, CER; x axis). The point for a trial in 
which the test treatment proves better than the control will be in the upper 
left of the graph, between the y axis and the line of equality. If test is no 
better than control then the point will fall on the line of equality.
 

The Figure shows results from trials of both antidepressants and 
anticonvulsants in diabetic neuropathy [McQuay et al, in press]. The most 
important feature to notice when thinking about future RSD studies is that 
the CER, the event rate in control groups, varies enormously, even when 
trials are showing similar levels of efficacy in the EER. This means that 
calculating sample size from a fixed CER of say 35% may grossly underestimate the numbers required.
 

GETTING A GRIP
Few of us sees enough RSD patients each year to mount studies of adequate 
size which will complete within our working lifetimes. The way forward has to 
be multi-centre studies which follow the design feature outlined by Tanelian 
and above, perhaps organised under the aegis of APS. Which question should we tackle first?
 

References
L'Abbé KA, Detsky AS. Meta-analysis in clinical research. Ann Intern Med 
1987; 107:224-33. 

McQuay H, Nye BA, Carroll D, Wiffen PJ, Tramèr M, Moore RA. A systematic 
review of antidepressants in neuropathic pain. Pain (in press) 

McQuay H, Carroll D, Jadad AR, Wiffen P, Moore A. Anticonvulsant drugs for 
management of pain: a systematic review. British Medical Journal 1995; 
311:1047-52. 
 
 

 

Back