|
Helmholtz used the principle of irradiation to explain the observation
that two squares (seen above) do not appear to be identical: The white
middle square appears larger than the black middle square. The term "irradiation"
refers to the spreading of light areas into adjacent dark areas, where
one would get an increase in the size of a bright area at the expense of
an adjacent dark area. This gets apparent displacement of a black-white
boundary so that the contour appears shifted in the direction of the dark
area. Helmholtz was rather vague on how this could occur.
Although Münsterberg first
proposed that irradiation could have a role in this illusion, he
offered no suggestion on how it could actually produce an angular distortion.
In recent years, however, many researchers have re-examined irradiation's
role in Münsterberg figure.
In 1980 Steven Taylor
and J. Margaret Woodhouse constructed a series of series of patterns in
an attempt to obtain effects similar to the Münsterberg figure but
with irradiation effects removed or reduced.
They found that if the
outline of the squares was overlapped so that one side was common to the
individual squares in adjacent lines, the lines appeared to be parallel.
However, if the squares were made to touch, rather than overlap, the effect
that was produced was similar to the Münsterberg figure, but with
the direction of tilt reversed. This illusion is known as the Taylor-Woodhouse
illusion or the hollow squares illusion.
Taylor and Woodhouse
found that the figure could be reduced to an even more basic form (Figure
still to come). Here, with only two parallel lines, the tilt remains
even after removal of the vertical and peripheral lines. The illusion disappears
with the removal of the overlapping lines. This illusion bears a strong
resemblance to the twisted cord illusion developed by Fraser.
In 1908, James Fraser, a British
psychologist, published a paper entitled "A New Visual Illusion of Direction."
He showed how one could adapt the Munsterberg figure into a new and powerful
illusion of direction -- the twisted cord.
Fraser called the basic
unit of his figures, a line with a triangle at either end, a "directional
unit." He maintained that this directional unit was very effective at misleading
the visual system.
Fraser used this unit
on both straight and curved figures, including the famous Fraser
Spiral.
Fraser noticed that it was
important for the individuality of the elements to be disguised, either
by overprinting the discontinuous adjacent ends of the tilted lines or
by placing the figure on a complex patterned background.
The importance of this fact is clearly
demonstrad by Fraser in his series of examples of the "LIFE" illusion.
Here the letters are
superimposed on a checkered background and the illusion is very strong.
However, when you remove
the background, the illusion is reduced.
REFERENCES:
Anstis, S. and C. Tyler, (1980) "Induced tilt
from checkerboards: Edges vs. Fourier components," Investigative Ophthalmology
and Visual Science, 19, 165
Bressan, P., (1985) "Revisitation of the family
tie between Münsterberg and Taylor-Woodhouse illusions," Perception,
14,
579-585
Coren, S. and J. S. Girgus, (1978) Seeing is
Deceiving: The Psychology of Visual Illusions, Hillsdale, New Jersey,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Cowan, T. M. (1973) "Some variations of the twisted
cord illusion and their analyses," Perception and Psychophysics,
14,
553-564
Day, R. H. (1978) "A note on the Münsterberg
or café wall illusion," Perception, 7, 123-124
DeValois, K. K. and R. L. DeValois, et. al. (1979)
"Responses of striate cortex cells to gratings and checkerboard patterns,"
London
Journal of Physiology, 291, 483-505
Earle, D. and S. Maskell, (1993) "Fraser cords
and the reversal of the café wall illusion," Perception,
22,
383-390
Fraser, J., (1908) "A new illusion of direction,"
British
Journal of Psychology, 2, 307-320
Gregory, R. L. and P. Heard, (1979) "Border locking
and the café wall illusion," Perception, 8, 365-380
Gregory, R. L. (1977) "Vision with isoluminant
color contrast: 1. A projection technique and observations," Perception,6,
113-119
Grossberg, S. and E. Mingolla, (1985) "Neural
dynamics of form perception: Boundary completion, illusory figures, and
neon color spreading," Psychological Review, 92, 173-211
Haig, N. (1989) "A new visual illusion, and its
mechanism," Perception, 18, 333-345
Luckiesh, M., (1922) Visual Illusions: Their
Causes, Characteristics and Applications, New York, Dover Publications,
Inc., 1965
Marr, D. and E. C. Hildreth, (1980) "A theory
of edge detection," Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,
Section B, 207, 187-217
McCourt, M. E., (1983) "Brightness induction and
the café wall illusion," Perception, 12, 131-142
Morgan, M. J. and B. Moulden, (1986) "The Münsterberg
figure and twisted cords," Vision Research, 26, no. 11, 1793-1800
Moulden, B. and J. Renshaw, (1979) "The Münsterberg
illusion and 'irradiation,'" Perception, 8, 275-301
Münsterberg, H., (1894) Pseudoptics,
New York, Milton Bradley
Münsterberg, H., (1897) "Die verschobene
Schachbrettfigur," Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 5, 185-188
Robinson, J. O., (1972) The Psychology of Visual
Illusion, London, Hutchinson University Library
Stuart, G. W. and R. H. Day, (1991) "The Fraser
illusion: Complex figures," Perception and Psychophysics, 49,
No. 5, 465-468
Stuart, G. W. and R. H. Day, (1988) "The Fraser
illusion: Simple figures," Perception and Psychophysics, 44,
No. 5, 409-420
Taylor, S., and J. M. Woodhouse, (1980) "A new
illusion and possible links with the Münsterberg and Fraser illusions
of direction," Perception, 9, 479-481
Wade, N., 1982. The Art and Science of Visual
Illusions, London, Rouledge & Kegan Paul
Woodhouse, J. M. and S. Taylor, (1987) "Further
Studies of the café wall and hollow squares illusions," Perception,
16,
467-471
|
|