Motivation and Learning: An Application of Theory to Social Studies
An Introduction to the Problem
Literacy and competency among graduate students from secondary education have
become the focus of attention in recent political discussions. This focus
usually points out the number of graduates who cannot function in a literate
society due to a lack of basic skills and knowledge. The focus tends to raise
the issues of remaining competitive in the global community and what can be done
to enhance the abilities of students in the educational system. The result of
this focus is a campaign to institute educational reform in hopes of raising
standards of achievement and literacy. Those involved in this debate tend to
focus on how things are being done in school systems and tend to promote
sweeping changes without specifying how these changes will improve achievement
scores.
Unfortunately, most of this debate, if it could be called that, has become more
characterized by accusations and name-calling than anything of substance. It
seems that everyone is trying to place the blame on educational failure on
specific individuals or institutional structures. Administrators and educators
are accused of being tied up in a bureaucracy in which the power to maintain the
position is all that matters. Others say that not enough money is being directed
to the educational needs of our children. Still others say it is because
teachers are incompetent. Rather than focusing on the learning process and
seeing if the problem might lie in the process, school reformists appear to be
demanding more discipline and structured instruction. The goal is usually stated
as getting back to the basics and of removing the incompetent highly unionized
teachers. This latter goal was potently exemplified in a letter to the editor in
the Mobile Press Register. Nicholas Jongebloed concludes her letter by stating,
“If we are really serious about strengthening our schools, we must first
realize what we face: a teacher population that is huge ( 2.5 million ) , their
test scores pathetic, their unions intransigent and powerful ( the single most
powerful force in the Democratic party ) and their tenure secure” ( 1995 ).
While I would not be so naive to say that there are no incompetent teachers, I
and most of the people I have associated with over the years have found teachers
to be competent, compassionate and interested in the welfare of their students.
To put down the teaching profession and suggest that the union is the reason for
all of educational problems we face is ludicrous. In addition, people who say
that more money is the answer to the problem have not examined the results of
the hundreds of millions of dollars that have been invested into the Kansas City
School District in Kansas City, Missouri. After millions of dollars were spent
on structural improvements, test scores and literacy have not been raised
significantly. Obviously, there are other issues that are more critical to
raising educational achievement.
I believe one critical issue that affects educational achievement is the lack of
motivation to learn among the student population. This problem has been pointed
out by many researchers who have found that many students are bored in school
and find no connection between school learning objectives and their personal
lives. Even more problematic for me as a social studies teacher, students have
for decades shown an aversion to social studies (Hootstein, 1994) . In order for
me to achieve the educational objectives that are enumerated in social studies,
I need to understand which factors are causing this lack of interest and what I
can do to enhance the learning of social studies. Simply throwing money at the
problem is not the answer. I can have the best technical tools available to
assist in teaching, but this in itself does not address the problem of why
students are not interested or motivated. Additionally, I rate myself as being
highly competent and very capable of teaching. Consequently, if money is not
necessarily the problem, and my competence is not the issue, what then is the
problem? Why are students not interested in the educational process and what can
be done to change this orientation?
The solution I propose is fundamentally a philosophical issue. The reason I call
it a philosophical issue is because I believe that the key to enhancing student
motivation and interest is a perspective that can encourage and stimulate
learning. This will be true for social studies as well as other content areas.
This perspective is not new and has been supported by volumes of research.
However, the perspective does represent a radical shift in the way
administrators, teachers and parents view the educational process.
Traditionally, the educational process has been characterized by a dependence on
behavioral modification with its emphasis on rewards and reinforcement. While I
would not totally disregard some aspects of this theory, I will show the
relevancy of seeing students as dynamic, curious and capable human beings who
learn all the time and are motivated. In this perspective, I see students as
free human beings who have a choice in what will attract his or her attention. I
also see the students as being actively engaged in trying to control their
environment and make sense of the world around them. Furthermore, by creating
the environment in which the student’s natural tendency to explore and
understand the world is enhanced, I believe it is possible to enhance student
learning and motivation. Interestingly enough, this does not require a massive
government subsidy.
Before examining an environment that can enhance learning and motivation, I must
detail the theoretical foundations upon which such an environment can be
constructed. The next section will be an elaboration of the theoretical
foundations of motivation and student learning. In this section, I will detail
results from research that explore the question of student motivation and how
learning is affected by motivation. Following this section, I will offer my
thoughts and analysis of what research has indicated. I will also offer
suggestions on how to construct an environment that is conducive to learning in
the Social Studies content area.
Defining Motivation
The first question to be addressed is whether there is any link between
motivation and learning. In order to answer this question, a definition of
learning and motivation must be identified. In simple terms, learning is defined
as “a relatively permanent change in behavior due to experience,” and as,
“a relatively permanent change in mental associations due to experience” (
Ormrod, 1995 ). In both of these definitions the important term is “permanent
change.” This simply means that as a teacher I know that learning has occurred
when a permanent change in a student's attitudes, thoughts or behaviors has
occurred. This change can be measurable, such as on an evaluation, or it can be
observed by the student’s behavior. Furthermore, learning can be defined as
“a potential for behavior - as habits ( or knowledge ) available for
execution” (Logan, 1976 ) . This simply implies that learning can take place
that may not be measurable or observable at the time of instruction. How many
times have we learned something that was not used or tested at the time we
learned it, but at some point in our lives it became useful to our comprehending
a new challenge or situation. Human beings are dynamic in their ability to
recall information that has relevancy to a new experience. We equally can take
prior knowledge from learning situations and apply them to new problems. The
question that remains is how are we motivated to take this prior knowledge and
apply it to new situations? What motivates an individual to act in a certain way
in a new learning environment? Given that with every day of life individuals are
presented with learning opportunities, what does motivation have to do with what
is learned and available for recall? Also, how are motivation and learning
connected?
To answer these questions, a definition of motivation must be identified. A
simplistic definition would suggest that motivation is to be viewed as the
internal drive that energizes or moves the individual to act in a certain
manner. Upon closer examination, however, defining motivation involves an
analysis of the more complex characteristics of human behavior. Motivation in
essence is not a quality of human beings that can easily be measured. Equally,
motivation for doing any particular task can change from moment to moment. In
order to arrive at a true understanding of motivation, I will examine how the
term has come to be viewed among various researchers.
Behaviorism and Motivation
Motivation for the most part of the 1900s was seen as internal drives within the
individual. These drives have been described as drive motivation and incentive
motivation. Drive motivation has been defined as the psychological energy that
drives individuals to satisfy biological needs. Drive motivation has been
further subdivided and defined as primary and secondary motivation. Primary
motivation refers to the drive to satisfy hunger, thirst, sex, or avoidance of
physical pain. Secondary motivation refers to the innate drives to reduce
anxiety, frustration, and fear ( Logan, 1976 ) . Secondary motivation is not
seen as being less important than primary motivation. The difference is that
secondary motivation is a learned response caused by the environment. For
example, when a child is misbehaving, a parent will tell the child he or she is
to get control of himself or herself or else a punishment will be incurred. The
child is then motivated to stop misbehaving in order to avoid the punishment.
Incentive motivation can be defined as the “anticipation of rewarding
stimuli” that causes an individual to behave in a certain manner ( Beck, 1990
) . Incentive motivation can be viewed as the basis for behavior modification.
The chief proponent of this theory is Burrhus Skinner. The two fundamental
points of this theory state: “A response that is followed by a reinforcing
stimulus is strengthened and therefore more likely to occur again,” and, “A
response that is not followed by a reinforcing stimulus is weakened and
therefore less likely to occur again” ( Ormrod, 1995 ) . From the research
that Skinner and others have undertaken, research that primarily deals with rats
and animals, behaviorists have deducted that behavior is controlled via the use
of rewards and punishments. The basic theme of this theory is “Do this and
you’ll get that” (Kohn, 1993 ) . As simple as this view may seem, this
perspective has come too dominant in not only the classroom, but also in every
aspect of our lives.
Both drive motivation and incentive motivation view the basis for motivation as
some stimuli in the environment or a physical need. Human beings are viewed as
organisms that either are driven to satisfy some physiological need or are
conditioned to react to the environment in a certain way due to past
experiences. In this perspective, motivation is seen as the mechanical drive
that causes performance. Learning can occur with or without motivation present.
Logan states this perspective this way, “...reward affects performance, not as
a learning variable, but as a motivational variable; that reward provides
incentive motivating the performance of habits”( 1976 ) . In educational
settings, performance can be increased by the use of rewards and other
incentives. Learning is thus indirectly affected by motivation.
This perspective was further refined by Abraham Maslow. Rather than looking at
motivation as being derived from the drive to settle physiological needs, Maslow
proposed a hierarchy of needs. This hierarchy consists of physiological needs,
safety needs, belongingness and love needs, esteem needs, and the need for
self-actualization. Maslow proposed that basic human needs are organized into a
“hierarchy of relative prepotency” ( Maslow, 1970 ) . By this he simply
means that as lower needs are meet, higher needs emerge and drive the individual
to meet these needs. For example, when an individual has all of his or her
physiological needs met, he or she will then be driven to satisfy safety needs.
When safety needs are met, the individual will be driven to satisfy esteem
needs, and so forth through the five divisions of the hierarchy. Motivation is
the drive to satisfy these varying needs as they arise. Learning can be seen as
habit development in the attempt to satisfy these needs. Whenever a new habit is
formed, it can be deduced that the individual has learned a new way to satisfy
the need that is facing him or her. Maslow also described the motivation to
satisfy these needs as being intrinsic in nature.
Given that the behavioral perspective has influenced educational techniques for
many years, it might be viewed as irresponsible for me to suggest that
behaviorism has had a negative impact on student learning and motivation. On the
surface, it appears that many students strive for the rewards they are offered
and behavioral modification has been proven to be effective by numerous studies.
Equally, I could not argue that when basic physiological needs of students are
not being met, such as the room being too hot or too cold, students' attention
and motivation are directed toward relieving these needs. If there is anything
politicians and administrators can do to promote academic achievement, it would
be to provide humane environments in which these basic needs are met and do not
become distractions to the learning process. However, behaviorism does not take
into account the dynamic nature of the human spirit. Students often strive for
higher needs when basic needs are not being met. Furthermore, behaviorism
focuses primarily on extrinsic rewards. Yet within every school setting, there
exists a number of students who seem driven or motivated by some internal factor
that is unrelated to the extrinsic rewards that are offered. To understand this
discrepancy, I will now turn to what has been defined as cognitive theory of
motivation.
Cognitivism and Motivation
Cognitivism is a perspective that accounts for many of the unanswered questions
I face in the quest to understand motivation. This perspective is also known as
an affective approach to teaching. The basic theory of cognitivism is “that
individuals give meaning to their experiences. While environmental influences
are not discounted..., the environment is seen as being acted upon rather than
causing human behavior” ( Grippin & Peters, 1984 ) . In this perspective,
students have control over what they will attend to and what stimuli in the
environment receive the most attention. Given the fact that individuals are
bombarded at any given moment with thousands of different stimuli, the fact that
we attend to or choose a specific stimulus suggests a degree of autonomy within
the individual. If this is the case, then the simple introduction of rewards or
punishment may not be effective or even produce the desired result.
Research to support this perspective has continued to be produced over the last
twenty years. Equally important about most of this research, it has primarily
involved the use of human beings, not rats and animals. Critical to an
understanding of this perspective is the difference between intrinsic motivation
and extrinsic motivation. Extrinsic motivation refers to the rewards that an
individual receives for performing some task. Intrinsic motivation, in a most
simplistic understanding, refers to being motivated to complete some activity
that carries no reward other than doing the activity itself. A more formal
definition of intrinsic motivation is offered by Edward Deci ( 1975 ) who is one
of the most prominent researchers of intrinsic motivation.
Intrinsically motivated behaviors are behaviors which a person engages in to
feel competent and self-determining....Intrinsically motivated behaviors will be
of two general kinds. When there is no stimulation people will seek it. A person
who gets no stimulation will not feel competent and self-determining: he will
probably feel “blah.” So he seeks out the opportunity to behave in ways
which allow him to feel competent and self-determining. He will seek out
challenge. The other general kind of intrinsically motivated behavior involves
conquering challenges or reducing incongruity ( or reduce dissonance, etc. ) and
only when a person is able to conquer the challenges which he encounters or
creates will he feel competent and self-determining ( 61 ).
This definition suggests that the cognitive perspective places more emphasis on
the individual and the control that the individual demonstrates over self and
the environment. If this type of control is positive and enhances motivation and
learning in the classroom, then clearly the desire to enhance intrinsic
motivation in students is a priority for the teacher. The question that remains
is how can intrinsic motivation be encouraged and what factors will reduce this
type of motivation? If intrinsically motivated students are the type who tend to
achieve academically and become life- long learners, then what kind of classroom
environment will enhance the possibility of intrinsic learning behaviors?
To answer these types of questions, researchers have focused on which conditions
enhance intrinsic motivation and which conditions reduce it. Research has
concentrated on how extrinsic rewards affect the intrinsic value of performing a
task. These studies suggest that when extrinsic rewards are used in connection
with performance, the intrinsic value of performing that activity is diminished.
In situations where autonomy and freedom of expression are provided, individuals
tend to display a higher degree of intrinsic motivation, as well as a higher
level of self-esteem and competence ( Deci & Ryan, 1992 ) . The reason for
this effect is theorized to be due to the nature in which the individual
perceives the activity. If the individual believes they are being pressured to
perform in a certain way, either by the promise of rewards or by the presence of
some evaluative standard, the environment is perceived to be controlling. The
individual is thereby less likely to take risks and becomes distracted by the
reward or evaluation. If, however, the individual perceives the environment as
supportive and offering a degree of autonomy, performance is more likely to be
intrinsically motivated. The individual perceives himself as having some control
and choice over the activity or learning process. In addition, in choosing how
to perform an activity, the individual is more likely to show initiative that
produces feelings of self-determination and competency. These feelings in turn
enhance the intrinsic value of the activity ( Deci & Ryan, 1992 ) .
The importance of this research for classroom instruction is clear. Teachers who
continue to view their position as one of controlling the growth of knowledge
and skills in their students may be using techniques and approaches that
diminish the value of the learning objective. The reason for a student
performing an activity comes to be based on some external purpose rather than
having true relevance or enjoyment for the student. The student thus resorts to
fulfilling some objective standard set by the teacher and becomes more dependent
on extrinsic motivations. This in turn, reduces the intrinsic motivation of the
student and the intrinsic value of the activity. This phenomenon is defined as
the overjustification effect by Lepper and Green ( 1978 ) . While some studies
have shown that this effect is not universal in application ( e. g. , highly
motivated students do not tend to have their intrinsic motivation diminished by
extrinsic rewards ) , the fact remains that studies have shown that the
motivational orientation of students is affected by the use of extrinsic
motivational techniques. Furthermore, research has shown that students who are
motivated by extrinsic rewards become characterized by a preference for easy and
predictable tasks. The desire to take risks and test different internal
solutions to problems are supplanted with the desire to achieve good grades or
to satisfy some extrinsic motivational technique.
Other researchers, noting that as students progress through grades they tend to
develop a more extrinsic motivational orientation, have tried to focus on the
effects of extrinsic motivational techniques in the classroom. These studies
have provided several important insights ( Harter, 1992 ) . First was a finding
that the focus on the evaluation of an individual’s competence influenced the
way the student perceived his competence. This, in turn, affected the
students’ motivational orientation. This focus on the assessment of an
individual’s ability and performance, rather than on the learning objective,
tended to diminish the student’s interest and motivation. A second finding was
that as students progress through the educational process, the focus on higher
grades and rewards for the products of their endeavors caused intrinsic
motivation to decrease. Being rewarded for simply being interested in a subject
or having an interest to learn does not occur in most educational settings. The
emphasis is on the reward of having achieved some standard set by the teacher.
This emphasis, by its nature, devalues the process of learning and focuses on
the goal ( Kohn, 1993 ) .
One final finding, that is relevant from this research, indicated that feelings
of competence directly influenced the type of motivation orientation that was
displayed by students. Before detailing the results of this research, I must
define a third type of motivation that was measured in this study called
internalized motivation. Internalized motivation referred to those behaviors and
attitudes that have moved from being produced by extrinsic motivational
techniques and have become internalized. These behaviors and attitudes are those
that the individual has come to accept as important from the socializing
context. Other researchers have further elaborated on internalized motivation by
describing it as self-regulation in the individual. An individual exhibiting
this motivation is viewed as lying along a continuum. At one end is external
regulation in which an individual performs a task to receive reward or avoid
punishment. A higher degree of internalization is called introjected regulation
in which individuals see their behavior linked to the approval of others. A
higher degree is described as identification in which the individual performs a
behavior due to his or her own choice or value. The highest form of
internalization is defined as integration in which the individual incorporates a
behavior into his or her system of value (Ryan, Connell, & Grolnick, 1992 )
.
Using a test instrument designed to measure the three types of motivation (
e.g., extrinsic, internalized, and intrinsic ) , Harper ( 1992 ) measured middle
school students' attitudes toward learning. The results of this study indicated
there were four distinctive patterns of motivational combinations. Type A
student was one who scored high on intrinsic motivation and internalized
motivation, but scored low on extrinsic motivation. Type B student scored high
on all three motivational factors. Type C student scored low on intrinsic
motivation and internalized motivation, but scored high on extrinsic motivation.
Type D student scored low on all three motivational factors. Further analysis of
the results revealed that students in the Type A profile were significantly
higher in feelings of competency. Accordingly, those students in the Type D
profile expressed the least amount of self-competence and had low expectations
of performance and ability. Those students in Types B and C showed a similar
incremental difference in feelings of competency. The significance of this study
to this analysis is that it reveals four distinctive motivational orientations
that the teacher may encounter. It also demonstrates that motivational
orientations are related to the student’s feelings of competency and ability.
In summary, many of the findings of the cognitive perspective tend to suggest
that the traditional orientation of teaching students needs to be scrutinized.
If the behaviorist perspective to motivation and learning is correct, my task as
a teacher is made much easier in that I can control the learning process by the
use of external reinforcements, such as good grades, praise, and other positive
stimuli. However, this appears to be a simplistic understanding of human nature.
The appealing aspect of the cognitive perspective is it tends to define the
nature of human motivation in terms that seem more believable to what I have
observed in life. Since studies have shown that the perspective I take as a
teacher can impact the learning process in as little as six weeks, it is
imperative that I choose a teaching style and philosophy that will enhance and
encourage learning in social studies. Given this choice, I will now turn to how
application of learning and motivational theory can be used in the construction
of a learning environment in which learning and intrinsic motivation can be
enhanced.
An Application of Theory to Social Studies Instruction
I began this paper with the question of how I could enhance the motivation and
interests of students to become interested to learn social studies. In order to
answer this question, I have had to first admit that I cannot motivate any
student. Assuming a cognitive perspective, I am not in control of the interests
and motivations of my students, nor would I want to claim that responsibility. I
do not want my students to be interested in social studies simply because it
fascinates me or because I have some reward to give them. Preferably, I would
rather have students who find the subject matter challenging and interesting.
Students who take responsibility for their learning. Students who want to come
to class and who want to contribute to the learning process. While this is the
ideal, and some would say a utopian ideal, I realize that there will be some
students who will never be interested in social studies or school. This is to be
accepted. However, since I view students as being in control of their
motivations and interests, or at least desiring to be in control of the things
they will or will not attend to, I realize there are techniques and attitudes
that I have control over that can create a learning environment in which
students are more likely to be motivated and interested in what is going on
around them. Key to creating this environment is to acknowledge the reality of
two very basic facts.
The first reality is that there will be immense pressure from outside the
classroom for me to perform to a set of academic standards. This particular
pressure can only be seen as intensifying as politicians, administrators and
parents try to push forward school reform. School reformists, in their quest to
reach higher standards of achievement, appear to be calling for more discipline,
stricter evaluations of teachers, and more accountability on the part of
teachers. While there may appear to be nothing negative about these ideals in
themselves, I know that this outside pressure or control can have an impact on
my teaching perspective. Studies have demonstrated that this type of pressure
can force teachers to adopt a more controlling perspective in teaching and to
stress rote memorization of knowledge over more desirable learning objectives (
Flink, Boggiano, Main, Barrett, & Katz, 1992; Sturtevant, 1994 ) . I feel
that many school reformists have no interest in whether children are motivated
or are becoming life-long learners, rather they just want to get achievement
scores higher, however it is done. There is also the abiding belief that
education is just the means to the end and that it is the end that is important.
School, therefore, is the burden a person must endure before they can be
accepted into society. Personally, I believe these attitudes are the result of
the dominant behaviorial perspective that has existed for so may years, rather
than a true picture of the way education could be perceived. Equally, since most
of the people who are involved in school reform have at one time attended many
of the systems they are attempting to reform, these reformists feel that it is
not the process that is wrong, but the problem of incompetent teachers or
administrators or the lack of money. If they have survived and become
contributing members of society, the problem must be one of personnel or money.
Nonetheless, I will be forced to face these attitudes and pressures in any
environment of learning I attempt to create. This means I will have to insulate
the classroom environment from these pressures and attitudes.
The second key to creating an enhanced learning environment is to respect and
acknowledge where each student’s motivational orientation exists. This means
acknowledging the various learning styles, motivational levels, and interests
that each student brings into the classroom. Sure it would be nice to stand in
front of the classroom and try to mold each student into what I want them to be
and what to learn, but I believe this in itself is an impossible task and one
that will only lead to frustration and burnout. It is also an acknowledgement
that I will not be able to help a few students, no matter how much I might
desire, during my tenure as a teacher. On a positive side, however, I believe
this acknowledgement will allow me to see each student as an individual and use
their uniquness and interests as ways of building a teacher-student
relationship. Returning to the four motivational orientations dicussed earlier,
my perspective is to accept the reality of seeing these types of motivational
levels and to attempt to use these orientations as keys to teaching strategies
rather than trying to create the perfect motivational orientation. No doubt
there is more risk involved in doing this, but I believe that in this type of
environment the student’s natural desire to feel competent and to take on new
risks or challenges will be enhanced. My perspective is seen more as walking
alongside the student rather than trying to pull and bribe them to my position.
In every human relationship, some common ground of co-existence can usually be
found. If this were not the case, human beings would have obliterated themselves
from the face of the earth thousands of years ago.
Given these two key realities, I am now prepared to express my thoughts on
developing an enhanced learning environment. First and most important, I do not
view this learning environment to be one based on the best technology and the
best physical surroundings. In fact, I believe the environment can be created
anywhere in which a learning process is occurring. This is not to suggest that
technology has no place. I will always value technology as a means of providing
more strategies and tools for learning. However, for meaningful learning to
occur, the best technology is not a prerequisite. The most important factor I
can think of to enhance learning and motivation is to provide an environment in
which challenges and risks can be undertaken and self-worth is not threatened.
This by definition places the emphasis on the learning process and not the
individual. Learning is seen as a process of failure and success. The goal is
not to penalize for stupid or wrong solutions, but to arrive at the best
solution.
In social studies, this type of environment can easily be facilitated due to the
nature of the content. In history, the question is not who discovered America,
but why did Columbus discover the New World. In geography, the question is not
which two oceans are on each side of America, but how did these oceans
contribute to the unique American experience and its rise to power. Answers to
these types of questions cannot be gained from simple memorization of facts.
They involve decision-making based on the facts. They involve the analysis of
facts and the ascribing of some value to these facts. These types of questions
require the student to consciously interact with the facts and draw conclusions
based on prior knowledge and one's value system. On an emotional level, these
types of questions require individuals to experience the significance of the
subject and respond from a level of commitment. When errors in judgment do
occur, the process should be seen as a part of problem-solving and not a
reflection of personal shortcomings ( Casey & Tucker, 1994 ) . I might add,
this environment is not one in which I will try to enhance student self-esteem
as a goal. The self-esteem movement in education seems to be an extreme
application of cognitive theory as others have noted ( Kohn, 1994 ) . The goal
is to put emphasis on the learning process and enhancing academic competency,
not the individual and how he or she feels about themselves.
Fortunately for myself, there are many different strategies that have been found
to be effective in producing this type of learning environment. In one study,
the types of activities that students said motivated them to learn included
role-playing, group discussion, watching relevant videos or films, playing games
and “giving students more sense of control” ( Hoostein, 1994 ) . Reading
content material also has been found to be more interesting when students have a
chance to share their thoughts and opinions on what they have read ( Davis, 1994
) . Another project showed that the use of graphic organizers or content mapping
allowed students to gain a sense of control over the learning content and
objectives and increased comprehension significantly ( Peresich, Meadows, &
Sinatra, 1990 ) . What all these strategies have in common is the central idea
of giving students more control over the learning process and allowing them to
be more involved in the acquisition of content knowledge. In addition to
developing higher categories of critical thinking, these types of activities
allow students to develop competency in their abilities and beliefs. This in
turn should help to enhance the intrinsic value of the content material and
expedite the learning process.
Having taken an informal poll of my son and some of his high school friends, I
found that the classes they liked the most were those in which these types of
activities were part of the learning process. In addition, most of the students
I talked to said they made better grades in this type of environment. I might
add, many of these students speak on other occasions as being bored with school
work. It is my conviction that these responses are reflective of the results
obtained by researchers working from a cognitive perspective. As students'
competency in the learning process grows, and their comprehension of the content
material becomes internalized into their personal belief systems and values, I
believe their intrinsic motivation will grow correspondingly. The result will be
a student who is capable of not only critical thinking skills, but who also has
an enriched experience of the world in which he or she participates.
One other important aspect of this teaching perspective is the use of grading.
Since grades have been the end for most instructional activity and for ranking,
I cannot accept the total abolishment of the grading system. However, I do
believe that a modification of its use is in order. Since so much research has
indicated the negative effects of grading and of putting the stress on this
aspect of academic life, I see no reason not to de-emphasize this in the
classroom. This can be accomplished in a number of ways. One is to rethink the
use of tests and evaluations. In a recent test construction project I purposely
designed the test to see if (1) the student had learned the objectives for the
lesson period, and ( 2 ) to see if I had adequately taught the material. The
first four questions, worth approximately the same amount each, were designed to
answer these questions. An extra credit question was offered, worth more than
any of the other test questions. The only requirement to receive points for this
question was that the students had to answer the first four questions in some
manner. The extra credit question, however, was the one question I felt was most
important to the lesson plan. It was a question developed to force the student
to give his or her thoughts and only required that students support their answer
with relevant lesson material. The goal was to encourage creativity and
originality. How could I penalize any student for attempting to display these
types of higher-order skills? My hope was that the test question would offset
the anxiety level of the students on later exams and also encourage them to
think critically. On this exam, I also would make a provision for wrong answers
to be corrected by the student and thus encourage the student to pursue the end
of problem-solving, the solution.
I have shared this test design at length because I believe there are many
creative ways to reduce the anxiety and emphasis of tests and grades. A recent
survey found that parents felt that the use of a written description of the
student’s progress or a checklist of what the student is able to do was very
useful over a grading system by a margin of 74% to 58%. This indicates that even
parents are looking for more meaningful information about their child’s
progress ( Elam, Rose & Gallup, 1994 ) . I thus can use more informal, yet
potentially more meaningful, evaluation systems to give feedback to students and
parents on learning progress. Will the type of teaching perspective I promote
raise the achievement scores of students that are so important to school
reformists? I believe they will. While this fact has not been tested widely, I
did find one study in which teacher strategies were tested to see what effect
they had on student achievement. Students in the more controlling learning
environments tended to adopt a more extrinsic orientation and also did more
poorly on achievement tests ( Flink, Boggiano, Main, Barrett & Katz, 1992 )
. It would appear, therefore, that a less controlling environment can impact
achievement scores that are the central concern of school reformists.
Conclusions
While I am sure there are many who would contest my conclusions, I feel I have
developed a workable philosophy toward the learning process and environment. I
am still early in my educational pursuit and have yet to actually stand in front
of a class. Nonetheless, I believe that proceeding from a cognitive perspective
is not only more humane but also more realistic. As my knowledge and experience
grow, this philosophical foundation will be modified and improved upon. I also
recognize the place of rewards and recognition in the learning process and would
never seek to withhold honor from those whom it is due. Perhaps the greatest
value of this philosophical foundation is that it will allow me to become a part
of the learning process, rather than controlling, and learn from those whom I
have the privilege to instruct. In the end, this is the greatest accomplishment
I could hope to achieve.
References
Beck, R. C. ( 1990 ) . Motivation: Theories and principles ( 3rd. ed. ) .
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Casey, M. B. & Tucker, E. C. ( 1994 ) . Problem-centered classrooms:
Creating lifelong learners. Phi Delta Kappan, 76 ( 1 ) , 139 - 143.
Davis, S. J. ( 1994 ) . Make reading rewarding, not rewarded. The Education
Digest, 60 ( 2 ) , 63 - 65.
Deci, E. L. ( 1975 ) . Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. ( 1992 ) . The initiation and regulation of
intrinsically motivated learning and achievement. In A. K. Boggiano & T. S.
Pittman ( Eds. ) , Achievement and motivation: A social-developmental
perspective ( pp. 9 - 36 ) . New York: Cambridge University Press.
Elam, S. M., Rose, L. C. & Gallup, A. M. ( 1994 ) . The 26th annual Phi
Delta Kappa/Gallup Poll of the public’s attitudes toward the public schools.
Phi Delta Kappa, 76 ( 1 ) , 41 - 62.
Flink, C., Boggiano, A. K., Main, D. S., Barrett, M. & Katz, P. A. ( 1992 )
. Children’s achievement-related behaviors: the role of extrinsic and
intrinsic motivational orientations. In A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman (
Eds. ) , Achievement and motivation: A social-developmental perspective ( pp.
189 - 214 ) . New York: Cambridge University Press.
Grippin, P. C. & Peters, S. C. ( 1984 ) . Learning theory and learning
outcomes: The connection. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.
Harter, S. ( 1992 ) . The relationship between perceived competence, affect, and
motivational orientation within the classroom: processes and patterns of change.
In A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman ( Eds. ) , Achievement and motivation: A
social-developmental perspective ( pp. 77 - 114) . New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Hoostein, E. W. ( 1994 ) . Motivating students to learn. The Clearing House, 67
( 4 ) , 213 - 216.
Jongeblood, N. ( 1994, February 8 ) Teachers’ standing not very surprising. (
Letter to the Editor ) . The Mobile Press Register, p. A10.
Kohn, A. ( 1993 ) . Punished by rewards: The trouble with gold stars, incentive
plans, A’s, praise and other bribes. New York: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Kohn, A. ( 1994 ) . The truth about self-esteem. Phi Delta Kappan, 76 ( 4 ), 272
- 283.
Lepper, M. R. & Greene, D. ( 1978 ) . The hidden costs of rewards.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Logan, F. A. ( 1976 ) . Fundamentals of learning and motivation ( 2nd ed. ) .
Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown Company.
Maslow, A. H. ( 1970 ) . Motivation and personality ( 2nd ed. ) . New York:
Harper & Row.
Ormrod, J. E. ( 1995 ) . Human learning ( 2nd ed. ) . Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Peresich, M. L., Meadows, J. D. & Sinatra, R. ( 1990 ) . Content area
cognitive mapping for reading and writing proficiency. Journal of Reading, 33 (
6 ) , 424 - 432.
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P. & Grolnick, W. S. ( 1992 ) . When achievement is
not intrinsically motivated: a theory of internalization and self-regulation in
school. In A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman ( Eds. ) , Achievement and
motivation: A social-developmental perspective ( pp. 167 - 188 ) . New York:
Cambridge University Press.
© CopyRight 2002 Scott R. Simpson