Animal Behaviour Problems



CONTENTS::

1/ INTRODUCTION

2/ ALPHA BEHAVIOURS; DOMINANT AND SUBMISSIVE BEHAVIOURS

3/

4/

5/

6/

7/

8/

9/ ADDITIONAL READING, BIBLIOGRAPHY, AND DISCLAIMER



1. Introduction

Before beginning to discuss human-animal interaction, there is a (major) misconception that needs to be addressed, because it underpins all behaviours-interaction with others and/or the environment. It is the concept of right brain and left brain people. There is no such thing – unless someone has a ‘split brain’ or the hemispheres are not joined because the nerves have not connected both sides together. It appears that this concept arose because it was found that some people with split brains functioned in society by utilizing one hemisphere. It was, therefore, assumed that all people were the same; until the advent of Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Positron Emission Tomography (PET) (alias: CAT scans; or colloquially with endearment: the human washing machine), electrophysical* studies. This non-invasive technology has spawned a plethora of research that investigates which parts of the brain are ‘activated’ during a particular situation. One example of the types of studies that are undertaken is interpreting ‘happy’ and ‘fearful’ faces – a study by Morris et al (1997).

In this study, Morris et al found that people activated or‘used’ the right medial temporal gyrus, right putamen, left superior parietal lobe and left calcarine sulcus when interpreting ‘happy’ faces, whereas the left amygdala and left periamygdaloid cortex were activated for interpreting fearful faces. There was no right activation. Therefore, this has implications when discussing dominant and submissive behaviours – regardless of species (intra- [within] and inter- [between]).
But when there is bilateral (both left and right) damage to the amygdala, other brain structures may be commissioned to interpret fearful faces: areas such as the cingulate gyrus, cerebellum and the (pre-)frontal lobes.


* electrophysical studies. These studies tend to measure changes in the electrical current – whether it is within the brain, and/or, for example: do changes within the brain affect changes in the skin’s electrical currents (galvanic skin response).


2. Alpha Behaviours; Dominant and Submissive Behaviours


Within research over the past few decades, there has been much discussion re: dominant and submissive behaviours within and between species. This debate has resulted in several anomalies such as: Dominance, as in the exhibition of ‘aggressive’ behaviours

1/ is not a universal phenomenon (see Syme, 1974; Francis et al, 1992) and therefore cannot be applied to all species

2/ is not always associated with circulating androgens (such as testosterone)

3/ is more often associated with low brain serotonin – a neurotransmitters found in many species for example: humans ( ), dogs (Reisner et al), monkeys (Uno et al, ) and rats

4/ can be present in humans (Dodge, 1980; Patterson, 1989; Loeber et al, 1991)), dogs (Beaver, 1983) and laboratory rats () before sexual maturity.

Furthermore, aggression in dogs is usually divided into eight subsections: territorial aggression, inter-male - , maternal - , dominance - , fear-elicited - , and pain - (Beaver, 1983). (The latter is usually associated with a medical condition.) To complicate matters, several types of aggression (in dogs) can be found simultaneously.

This raises the question when describing dominance in animals of any age from infant to adult – at what stage(s) does one consider neurotransmitters, neural structure, and/or interaction as separate entities?

Another way to approach the issue of 'dominance' is to apply ecological theory in terms of social organization (Colinvaux, 1978) or social status between two or more individuals. This allows for the interaction of

1/ social organization within the species,

2/ social organization between other species; such human-companion animal,

3/ the potential display of aggression,

4/ the neurotransmitters steroids and neural structure present within each individual’s body and brain,

and

5/ the ages of the interacting individuals

without confounding the issue of what is ‘dominance’ and how can it measured consistently within and between different species; especially when each individual within a species responds to different situations due to different anatomical and physiological parameters (for example predator-prey relationships, human-horse interaction; dog-dog). Therefore, for the purposes of this discussion, social status will be defined as the display of alpha behaviours, or exhibiting dominant and submissive behaviours.

ALPHA BEHAVIOURS

DOMINANT BEHAVIOURS
As mentioned above, dominant behaviours involving aggression (implying the need for controlling others through threatening/intimidating/interrogating behaviours) are often associated with low serotonin within the brain. This is the fundamental reason why many aggressive dogs are euthanized – the unpredictability of when the dog’s brain will have low serotonin and the dog attacks another dog and/or human; at times without provocation.

SUBMISSIVE BEHAVIOURS
On the other hand, submissive behaviours are associated with several factors: reduced hippocampal volume, depleted norepinephrine (noradrenaline) and/or the advent of abuse/trauma.















9. Additional Reading, Bibliography, and Disclaimer


ADDITIONAL READING

*********************************************************************
BIBLIOGRAPHY

Beaver, B.V. Clinical classification of canine aggression. Applied Animal Ethology, 1983:10(1-2):35-43.

Beaver, B.V. Mental lapse syndrome. Journal of the American Animal Hospital Assoication, 1980:16(6):937-939.

Colinvaux, P. Why Big Fierce Animals are Rare. New Jersey, Princeton University Press.

Dodge, K.A. Social cognition and childre's aggressive behavior. Child Development, 1980:51(1):162-170.

Francis, R.C. On the relationship between aggression and social dominance. Ethology, 1988:78:223-237.

Francis, R.C, Jacobson,, B., Wingfield, J.C. & Fernald, R.D. Castration lowers aggression but not social dominance in male Haplochromis burtoni (Chichlidae). Ethology, 1992:90:247-255.

Loeber, R., Lahey, B.B. & Thomas, C. Diagnostic conundrum of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1991:100(3):379-390.

Morris, J.S., Frith, C.D., Perrett, D.I., Rowland, D., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J. & Dolan, R.J. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expresions. Nature, 1996:383(6603):812-815.

Patterson, G.R., DeBaryshe, B.D. & Ramsey, E. A developmental perspective on antisocial beahvior. American Psychologist, 1989:44(2):329-335.

Reisner, I.R., Erb, H.N. & Houpt, K.A. Risk factors for behavior-related euthanasia among dominant-aggressive dogs. 110 cases (1989-1992). Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Assoication, 1994:205(6):855-863.

Syme, G.J. Competitive orders as measures of social dominance. Animal Behaviour, 1974:22(4):931-940.

Uno, H., Tarara, R., Else, J.G., suleman, M.A. & Sapolosly, R.M. hippocampal damage associated with prolonged and fatal stress in primates. Journal of Neuroscience, 1989:9(5):1705-1711.

Wingfield, J.C. Control of territorial aggression in a changing environment. Psychoneuroimmunolgy, 1994:19(5-7):709-721.


barnesandnoble.com Home Page

*********************************************************************
DISCLAIMER:THE PURPOSES OF THIS PAGE IS FOR EDUCATIONAL PURPOSES ONLY.



Update: 19/06/00
01/01/02
29/01/02
Last update: 16/02/02

Author: JM




TARGET="_top">