Community for Women
Bisexual Professional Women in Australia

Home
Our Stories
Articles
Research
Meet Your Local Communities
Recommended Links
Products
Reviews



Bisexuality: A Contemporary Paradox for Women
new
 

Save a personal copy of this article and quickly find it again with Furl.net. It's free! Save it.

Paula C. Rodriguez Rust [*]

The cultural construction of "lesbian" and "heterosexual" women in late-nineteenth-century European cultures created both the possibility of conceiving the "bisexual" woman and the belief that bisexuality cannot exist. Social scientists have suggested several alternatives to dichotomous constructions of sexuality to facilitate the conceptualization of and therefore empirical research on, bisexuality. This article reviews these alternatives and summarizes the current state of research on bisexuality, including research on "situational homosexuality" (behavioral bisexuality), recent national probability studies on sexual behaviors and identities in the United States, the meanings of bisexual self-identities among women, masculinist biases in methods of assessing and theorizing sexual self-identities, and prejudice against bisexuals. The article concludes with suggestions for future social scientific research on bisexuality.

As we end the second millennium and begin the third, bisexuality is both uniquely conceivable and uniquely inconceivable in Western culture. This paradoxical position is the result of larger social and cultural factors that have shaped not only modern bisexuality but modern sexuality in general. Understanding bisexuality, therefore, is a key to understanding the cultural and historical factors that have affected not only bisexual but also lesbian and heterosexual women. In this article, I briefly describe the historical changes that produced the contemporary bisexual paradox, and I show how contemporary attitudes toward bisexuality result from this paradox. I then review social scientific efforts to reconceptualize bisexuality for the purposes of scientific study and summarize empirical research pertinent to bisexuality among women, including research on "situational homosexuality," the prevalence of bisexual behavior and identity in the United States, the meanings of bisexual self-identities and the ways in which women use sexual self-identities, racial and ethnic differences in patterns and meanings of bisexuality, and evidence of prejudice against bisexuals among heterosexuals and among lesbians and gay men (see also Rust, 1999, 2000).

Historical Paradoxes

Prior to the development of the concepts of the "lesbian" and the "heterosexual woman" as distinct types of people in the late nineteenth century, women in European and European-derived cultures were defined primarily by their familial relationships with husbands and children (Katz, 1995). Marriage served primarily economic and procreative functions rather than emotional functions, and women were expected to form their closest emotional bonds with other women (Smith-Rosenberg, 1975). Even if and when these bonds became sexual, women were not seen as "lesbians" because of their same-sex activities nor as a "bisexual" because of their simultaneous marriages to men, but as "women" because of their familial relationships with husbands and children. Thus, the tacit practice of bisexuality coexisted with the nonexistence of a concept of a (bi)sexual individual.

The late-nineteenth-century shift toward viewing women and men as eroticized individuals produced not only lesbians and heterosexual women, but also the possibility of conceptualizing bisexuality as a combination of lesbianism and heterosexuality. The gendered nature of the distinction between lesbianism and heterosexuality was critical in producing this possibility. If the newly eroticized individual had not been classified according to the gender to which she was attracted, the idea that she could be attracted to both genders would be unnecessary and nonsensical. The paradox lies in the fact that the same nineteenth-century beliefs in the mutual exclusivity of womanhood and manhood and in the inescapable importance of gender that produced concepts of gendered eroticism also produced the belief that sexual attraction must be directed toward either men or women. If men and women are "opposite" genders, then attractions toward women and men must also be opposite attractions that cannot coexist simultaneously within a single individual. If one is attracted to a man, how can one simultaneously be attracted to a woman who is everything a man is not and nothing that he is?

Ironically, therefore, the construction of lesbianism and heterosexuality pulled the rug out from under bisexuality. Whereas women in the nineteenth century might have enjoyed some freedom of bisexual expression in a culture that did not conceive of lesbians or heterosexuals, let alone bisexuals, the contemporary belief that lesbians and heterosexuals do exist has led to the possibility of conceptualizing bisexuality while also producing the belief that bisexuality cannot exist and thereby eroding the cultural space available for women's bisexual expression. The factors that have created this bisexual paradox are the same factors that have created contemporary lesbianism and heterosexuality. Understanding bisexuality among women, therefore, has the potential to shed light not only on the sexuality of bisexual women but also on the sexuality of lesbian and heterosexual women.

Contemporary Cultural Attitudes Toward Bisexuality

One of the greatest challenges facing bisexual women in contemporary Western culture is the belief that bisexuality does not exist. Women who claim to be bisexual are often told that they are "denying" their true sexuality, which must be either lesbian or heterosexual. Some young women who seek sexual experiences with other women are pegged as heterosexuals who are merely "experimenting" with women because lesbianism is chic. Other women, especially women who participate in lesbian communities but identify themselves as bisexual, are told that they are really lesbians who have not yet realized it because they are still coming out. If they continue to claim they are bisexuals, they are often accused of knowing they are really lesbians but purposefully denying it to avoid others' prejudices or to avoid sharing the burden of struggling against heterosexism. Bisexuality is sometimes seen as a cop-out or a way to get the "best of both worlds" without having to commit oneself to a particular lifestyle or a particu lar partner (e.g., Esterberg, 1997; Rust, 1993).

In addition to disbelief, bisexuals encounter many stereotypes about their sexuality. Because attractions to women and men are culturally constructed as contrary to each other, bisexuals are thought to be internally conflicted, emotionally or psychologically immature, or otherwise unstable. Bisexuals are also stereotyped as "needing" both male and female sex partners, as incapable of monogamy because they cannot be satisfied by only one partner, and as very sexually active. The cultural logic is as follows: A heterosexual's partner must be other-sex because s/he cannot be satisfied by a same-sex partner, and a lesbian or gay man's partner must be same-sex because s/he cannot be satisfied by an other-sex partner. Therefore, bisexuals must need both other-sex and same-sex partners to satisfy, respectively, the "heterosexual side" and the "lesbian/gay side" of their sexualities. In truth, few bisexuals have both female and male partners simultaneously (Rust, 2000), and even fewer feel that they "need" both fema le and male partners to be bisexual (Rust, in press). Just as an individual who appreciates both blue and brown eyes might be satisfied with either a blue-eyed or a brown-eyed lover without feeling a need for both, many bisexuals do not feel that their bisexuality requires them to be sexually active with both women and men simultaneously.

Reconceptualizing Sexuality to Create Space for Bisexuality

How, then, might we conceptualize bisexuality? Social scientists, pointing out that dichotomous conceptions of sexuality have led to a neglect of bisexuality in sex research, have offered numerous alternative models of sexuality. As noted by Rothblum (this issue), the best known of these alternatives is the 7-point scale proposed by Alfred Kinsey and his colleagues (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948), which ranges from exclusive heterosexuality, through degrees of bisexuality, to exclusive homosexuality. Other theorists have proposed using multiple Kinsey-type scales to represent, respectively, sexual attractions, sexual behaviors, sexual identity, and other aspects of sexuality (e.g., Bell & Weinberg, 1978; Shively, Rudolph, & De Cecco, 1978; Weinberg, Williams, & Pryor, 1994). The best-known modification of the Kinsey scale is the Klein Sexual Orientation Grid, in which individuals rate themselves three times on each of seven dimensions of sexuality; the three ratings reflect the individual's past, present, and "ideal" selves (Klein, 1993; Klein, Sepekoff, & Wolf, 1985).

Comment by Naomi De Plume

Whilst I enjoyed and appreciated this piece by renowned researcher Paula C. Rodriguez Rust, at the same time I felt quite incensed at the trite analogy of those bisexuals who need a male and female partner, as a choice as trivial as eye colour!  I assure readers that a woman and a man are very much more than their eye colour or their hair colour.  A far more apt analogy, giving true respect to the force of one's own sexual nature, is for some bisexuals, like being asked which hand they should have chopped off, without anaesthetic!

As someone who continues to live the pain of monogamy, despite a nature that is duogamous, I am qualified to make that statement.

Sexual preference is not, for most people, a product of their environment.  More and more, modern research, including brain imaging, shows that it is innate, and a product of a person's own genetic makeup and brain anatomy.  We see that biological gender, even in the case of intersex people and those with chromosome "abnormalities", does not indicate sexual preference.  It is far more complex than that.

It is also not something that can be changed, because it is, literally, hard-wired.  Sexual preference is not a conditioned response, but an unconditioned response.  Denying it causes damage not only to the individual, but also to society.

Whilst I applaud Rust's very valuable contributions, I must also loudly take issue with statements that trivialise and even denigrate the experience of bisexuals who genuinely need a male and a female in their lives.



 

Copyright © 2006 Bisexual Women in Australia
Last modified: 10/07/06